Real-Life Monsters

Real-Life Monsters

book review For Friday
Home
Psychology homework help
book review For Friday
Real-Life Monsters Real-Life Monsters

This page intentionally left blank

Real-Life Monsters Real-Life Monsters A Psychological Examination of the Serial Murderer

Stephen J. Giannangelo

Copyright 2012 by Stephen J. Giannangelo

All rights reserved. No part of this publication may be reproduced, stored in a retrieval system, or transmitted, in any form or by any means, electronic, mechanical, photocopying, recording, or otherwise, except for the inclusion of brief quotations in a review, without prior permission in writing from the publisher.

Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data

Giannangelo, Stephen J. Real-life monsters : a psychological examination of the serial murderer / Stephen J. Giannangelo. p. ; cm. Includes bibliographical references and index. ISBN 978-0-313-39784-4 (hardcopy : alk. paper) — ISBN 978-0-313-39785-1 (eISBN) I. Title. [DNLM: 1. Homicide—psychology. 2. Criminal Psychology. WM 605] 616.89—dc23 2012014963

ISBN: 978-0-313-39784-4 EISBN: 978-0-313-39785-1

16 15 14 13 12 1 2 3 4 5

This book is also available on the World Wide Web as an eBook. Visit www.abc-clio.com for details.

Praeger An Imprint of ABC-CLIO, LLC

ABC-CLIO, LLC 130 Cremona Drive, P.O. Box 1911 Santa Barbara, California 93116-1911

This book is printed on acid-free paper

Manufactured in the United States of America

For Mom and Dad – together again together always And for Kathy thank you for finding me

This page intentionally left blank

Contents

Preface ix

Acknowledgments xiii

PART I: THE DEVELOPMENT OF A SERIAL MURDERER

1. Introduction: An Identification of the Offender 3

2. Clinical Diagnoses and Serial Killer Traits 13

3. Biology and Its Effect on Violent Behavior 33

4. Environment, Background, and Personality 43

PART II: TOWARD AN EXAMINATION OF A THEORY OF VIOLENCE

5. Theoretical Discussion and Methodology 63

6. Case Studies 71

Case 1: Andrei Chikatilo 72

Case 2: Arthur Shawcross 77

Case 3: Jeffrey Dahmer 82

Case 4: Edmund Kemper 86

viii Contents

Case 5: Anthony Sowell 91

Case 6: Aileen Wuornos 96

Case 7: Dennis Rader 100

Case 8: Rodney Alcala 104

7. Reflecting on Case Studies 109

8. Theoretical Analysis and Development 121

9. A Conversation with a Monster? 129

10. Conclusion 143

Appendix A: Case Briefs 159

Appendix B: Glossary/Explanation of Terms 169

References 173

Index 185

Preface

In the early 1960s, as a child growing up in New England, I spent a great amount of time with a riveted focus on the subject of movie monsters. Plastic models, magazines, and the latest horror flick ruled the day for this child. I even managed to convince my slightly skeptical mother to allow me to go to bed early on Saturday evenings, so she could wake me up in time to watch the weekly monster movie shown at midnight. Concerned relatives counseled my mom, thinking that maybe I could turn out, well, odd. Maybe they were right.

Still, for some reason, I never was all that affected by the horrors of the cinema. While interesting, I never did buy into the terror of Frankenstein’s Monster, The Wolf Man, or the Creature from the Black Lagoon . They were interesting, but not real. Later movies came closer: The Exorcist, The Omen — these monsters looked a little more like they could be hiding under my bed—but not really. I believe this is what sparked my interest in human psy- chopathology and extreme abnormal psychology. I wanted to know what real monsters were capable of.

For it’s the people who walk among us that strike fear into our hearts. Not the seven-foot tall hulk with bolts in his neck, sewn together with leftover parts from the graveyard—it’s the normal-looking scrawny kid who works at the chocolate factory, brings people home, and desperately clings to the idea they might not leave this time. And then eats them. It’s not the half-man, half-wolf that howls at the full moon and scours the night for victims—it’s the chubby ex-shoe salesman turned contractor, the part-time performing clown, who lures a parade of young, unsuspecting victims to his home. And then buries them in his crawl space.

These are the monsters of real life. This is what real nightmares are made of. In a 1993 New York Times interview, prolific true-crime author Jack Olsen

recalled:

x Preface

we took a field trip to Holmesburg Prison, and I’m 19 years old and we get inside and I see all these guys who look just like me. I thought that criminals looked different. And this is what I attribute my interest in crime to.

I start every book with the idea that I want to explain how this seven or eight pounds of protoplasm went from his mommy’s arms to become a serial rapist or serial killer. This is my drive and my compulsion. I think a crime book that doesn’t do this is pure pornography.

I had the honor of discussing such issues with Mr. Olsen in 1997. He told me how much he liked my original book about serial murder and said it “was one of the clearer voices” on the subject and taught him plenty. This is high praise from an author I respected immensely. I’d hoped to have Mr. Olsen pen a foreword for this project, but sadly we lost him in 2002. He will be missed. I did need, however, to restate his position about curiosity based on that “seven or eight pounds of protoplasm.” I share this compulsion.

This book is a continuation—a 2012 perspective on an idea I developed in my first book, The Psychopathology of Serial Murder: A Theory of Violence. It seems the concept stands today, with over a decade’s worth of additional information and feedback. In addition, the perspective of this book, unlike those that cover the general subject of serial murderers, is to look specifically at the psychological underpinnings of these monsters that walk among us. The behaviors of these creatures are actually consistent enough to identify, and the goal is to pin down a developmental process that creates them.

The suggestion and presentation of an original theory is, at best, optimistic and ambitious. To attempt to explain a phenomenon such as the psychopa- thology of the serial killer could easily be described as overwhelming. How- ever, that is the intent of this book, as it was 15 years ago.

The reader must be aware that this book will by no means insinuate an explanation that could be carved in stone. The theory must be considered a starting point, a conclusion based on a limited amount of available knowl- edge; something to be reexamined and rethought with the passage of time and accumulation of information. Thus, this book continues that process.

The intent is to observe the available information regarding the serial killer, to dissect and apply the consistencies, and to develop a model of pathology. Observed will be clinical viewpoints, existing theory, socio-environmental in- fluences, and actual case histories. All these factors and more are critical to this analysis and will contribute to the thinking about this subject.

The serial murderer is examined in the context of the fourth edition, text revi- sion Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders ( DSM-IV-TR ), the reference published by the American Psychiatric Association. This is not to say that this book speaks only to those in the arena of psychology; quite the contrary. The book encompasses the disciplines of sociology, history, biology, psychology, and law enforcement, to name a few, and attempts to reach anyone interested in the development and the impact of this particular offender on our society.

Preface xi

This book is organized as a review of current clinical perspectives that could be applied to the psyche of the serial killer (Chapter 2); a review of the idea behind biological bases for violence, criminal behavior, and serial mur- der (Chapter 3); a discussion of sociological observations and environmental factors in the killers’ backgrounds (Chapter 4); a discussion of the synthesis of a preliminary theoretical model of pathology (Chapter 5); the analyses of eight case studies in the context of prior assumptions (Chapters 6 and 7); a resulting analysis of an etiological theory (Chapter 8); a fascinating, frank conversation I had with a confessed serial killer sentenced to death row and his insights (Chapter 9), and a conclusion (Chapter 10) with observations and suggestions. Future research is also considered.

The reader should note that references to various serial killers and related nonserial cases will appear throughout the text. For those unfamiliar with a particular case, a brief synopsis of each offender appears in alphabetical order in the Case Briefs of Appendix A. Therefore, each individual will not be foot- noted; they are easily referred to in the appendix.

The reader should also note that terms in this book are by no means uni- versal. Various writers have very specific usages for concept terms such as dissociative and deviance, which very well may not agree with others’ usage. Several forms of media use certain terms in a completely generic manner, to the point of losing their intended distinctions. For example, many newspapers and other media continue to use the term mass murderer in reference to serial killers, whereas those who study these individuals clearly do not find the terms interchangeable.

Therefore, the reader should direct his or her attention to the Glossary in Appendix B to clarify the use and intention of many of the terms used in this book. They are specifically used within the context of this book’s perspective and do not intend to insinuate a universal acceptance of their use.

The reader of this new volume should also be aware that while there are many new and current references, there are a number of older citations of studies and research done many years ago. Some are essential to the historical context and the discussion at hand; many were produced around the same time frame a certain offender was found out and prosecuted; others are classic references that remain important and relevant. I would actually call them es- sential, when discussing a narrowly viewed and examined subject such as the serial murderer and theories of violence and criminology. The reader should be assured that this work, while retaining much of the classic contributions of the genre, has clearly been reexamined in light of new study, theory, and science.

Clearly, theory about serial killer development will continue to rage. Argu- ments of guilt and innocence will continue, as in the cases of Wayne Williams, Albert DeSalvo, and Lucy de Berk. Over the years, long-established kill totals evolve as they have with the 2011 victim analysis of John Wayne Gacy. Even the identities of some killers, as famous as the Zodiac Killer and Jack the Rip- per, are unknown. “Nobody will ever know—I am as mystified now as I was

xii Preface

then,” said officers in the Ripper case, according to Honeycombe (2009). I’ve been lucky enough to view the Black Museum at Scotland Yard, and while some I spoke to were reasonably sure of their belief of Jack’s identity, others still did not agree. I just hope this book can aid the readers in making their own informed opinions about these debates and many more.

Maybe we can yet capture the real-life monsters they can’t quite convert to the movie screen.

Acknowledgments

The production of a new book is impossible without a host of great friends and respected people who offer input, guidance, and support.

To my friend Cathy Clevenger, my resident attachment disorder expert and long-time professional dealing with the most high-risk young offenders, a great amount of thanks for years of teaching knowledge, expertise and shared experiences, and a fine contribution to this project.

Michael Wilt and Valentina Tursini both contributed greatly to the final stage of making this new book possible. Michael for his help at the beginning and Valentina’s professional and tireless efforts in dealing with both my fuzzy proposal and endless questions throughout. And of course Beth Ptalis for tak- ing us to the finish line.

Kate Vernor, for suggestions and references on the research side. Professor Scott Culhane of the University of Wyoming and Commander

Ron Freeman of Duquesne University, for unselfishly offering some terrific research material, and Ron: thanks for the fine quote.

Great thanks to Jessica Senn, who graciously offered her expertise on the line art and figures.

I really should acknowledge Rick, who provided personal insight and an honesty about some difficult issues that must be recognized. These things are rarely discussed in this way.

I still must continue to acknowledge the assistance of those who made contributions to my previous book, without which this work would be impossible. Great thanks still go out to: Karen Kirkendall, Joel Adkins, Ron Ettinger, Karen Klainsek, Judy Rodden, Kim Egger, Dale Lael, Mike Giannangelo, Keith Hanson, Danielle Waller, Bob Craner, Ken Daugh- erty, Irene Kelly-Pasley, and my friend, mentor, and colleague, Dr. Steven Egger.

xiv Acknowledgments

And a massive, heartfelt thanks to my amazing wife Kathy Giannangelo, who not only provided unwavering support when I truly thought I’d never get to take a fresh look at this project, but also took on some difficult proof- reading and provided additional creative input. This is not to mention her essential IT skills and technical expertise that saved me endless hours of kick- ing computers and developing further alternate forms of linguistic profanity.

Part I Part I

The Development of a Serial Murderer

This page intentionally left blank

Introduction: An Identifi cation of the Offender

1

We continue to be haunted with the fear that people will remem- ber this killer, even glorify him, for the slaughter of our children.

—Families of victims of Danny Rolling

As I peered through the thick glass separating us, I caught a glimpse of my own reflection. I had a look of rapt attention, as if I could not wait to see what happened next. For a moment I was aware of the sad, grey prison walls enveloping us and the stark bleakness of the surround- ings. I glanced down and noticed I was literally on the edge of my seat, anticipating what he was about to say, in what direction he might go. My seat was quite a bit different than the stool he sat on, with hardware for shackles attached.

He explained to me why he killed so many women in a short pe- riod of time. He quickly agreed he’d do it again if given the chance. He debated whether he should really be considered a psychopath. As he spoke, I couldn’t help but think there was nothing about him that would make me consider him a threat if we were on the outside. He tried to understand what would take him from an adventurous young man to what people regarded as a monster.

I’m still trying to understand.

4 Real-Life Monsters

Serial murder. Rarely a day goes by without a fleeting reference to this expres- sion. It’s heard on television, in movies, in newspapers, and in everyday con- versation. More average Americans know who Jeffrey Dahmer is than Jonas Salk. Everybody’s heard of serial killers.

“We fear evil, but we are fascinated by it. We reject the ‘Other’ as different and dangerous because it’s unknown, yet we are thrilled by contemplating sexual excess and violations of moral codes by those who are not our kind” (Zimbardo, 2007, pp. 4–5).

But who are they? What makes a handsome, normal-appearing young law student murder dozens of women? Why does a mild-mannered chocolate fac- tory worker kill, dismember, and cannibalize individuals he found attractive, who came home with him on particular nights? What makes a serial killer kill? More important, what makes him or her in the first place?

The subject of serial murder has been the focus of an inordinate amount of attention, research, debate, and often exploitation in recent years. The sensationalism and emotional response the subject evokes make it an easy target for articles, movies, true crime books, yellow journalism, TV tabloids, and other money-making paraphernalia ranging from comic books to trading cards. However, sifting through this avalanche of sensationalism does yield a lining of true research and scientific study that might suggest answers to just who the serial killer is, and why.

SOME DEFINITIONS OF SERIAL MURDER The first difficult question is, who exactly is a serial killer? The Federal Bu-

reau of Investigation (FBI) has one definition; various theorists have others. Is it just someone who kills more than one person? A hit man? Is a merce- nary a serial killer? What about an abortion doctor? The more the question is asked, the more answers there seem to be.

Credit for the term “serial killer” is given by Ramsland (2005b) to Richard Hughes in his 1950 book, The Complete Detective . Robert Ressler, a long- time FBI profiler, is often credited with common usage of the term, after hearing the term series in use in England regarding a pattern of crime. He also felt this was comparable to serial adventures in the movie industry (Vronsky, 2004). Different writers and theorists have also quoted other examples of the use of the term in the 1960s and 1970s, while even more have claimed the credit themselves.

Many definitions of serial murder are established by specific parameters, such as victimology, geographic location, and killer–victim relationships. Some feel serial killers must reveal a certain pattern or their victims must rep- resent deeply rooted symbolism. Other theories include sex and dominance.

The FBI states their definition of serial murder, taken from Title 28, Sec- tion 540B of the U.S. Code, describes the killing of a series of three victims or more, “having common characteristics such as to suggest the reasonable pos- sibility that the crimes were committed by the same offender or offenders.”

Introduction: An Identifi cation of the Offender 5

According to Brantley and Kosky (2005), the agency makes no reference to “underlying motivation” and the definition is “intentionally broad” in order “to encompass the full array of serial killers” (p. 28).

In 2011, the FBI’s Behavioral Analysis Unit (BAU) published the results of a 2005 symposium held in San Antonio, Texas, comprised of 135 scholars, government crime experts, and veteran homicide detectives. This group con- cluded that serial murder should be defined by the “unlawful murder of two or more victims by the same offender in separate events.” This clearly broad defi- nition was intentionally designed out of concern that serial murders were being underestimated (Hargrove, 2011). However, this definition would incorrectly include gang killings, murder for hire, and spree killers, to name a few.

Very specific parameters cause some researchers to eliminate some of- fenders from a grouping of serial killers based on, possibly, a technicality. Colin Wilson, referring to G. J. Schaefer, stated, “technically speaking, based solely on his convictions, Schaefer could not be labeled a serial killer” because Schaefer had only been convicted of two killings (Schaefer & London, 1997, p. 1). Wilson apparently rigidly adheres to a three-victim minimum standard. It would be difficult to limit those studied as serial killers based on an arbitrary number of kills actually resulting in conviction.

Holmes and DeBurger (1988) listed distinctions they found within a pat- tern of serial murder. They included almost always a male killer and a female victim; a victim the same race as the killer; a killer whose age is between 25 and 35; no geographic variation in sites; victims of similar status; and a stranger-to-stranger relationship between killer and victim (p. 24). Inasmuch as these variables appeared consistent in their small (44) sample, most of these distinctions are arguable in light of various case histories. For example, while male killer or female victim roles are common, many cases include homo- sexual male killers and victims. A reasonable modification of this assump- tion could be male killers and eligible sexual partner victims. The common assumption that killers and victims are usually of the same race can also be disputed. While the general criminological principles state “murderers and victims look remarkably alike” and “murders tend to be overwhelmingly in- traracial” (Goode, 2008, p. 153), this seems to be more of a reflection of another conventional wisdom, that the likelihood of murder rises along with the level of intimacy between people. However, serial murder is more often a stranger-to-stranger crime, and too many of the cases tend to have unique features, which causes conflicts in painting with too broad a brush.

Eric Hickey, author of Serial Murderers and Their Victims (1991, p. 8), feels a serial killer must be defined in the most general of terms, so as not to eliminate offenders by speculation rather than through verifiable evidence. His definition includes “any offenders who kill over time.” Hickey’s assertion seems plausible for the most part, except that his all-inclusive population is not usable for purposes of this book. Hickey’s demographic approach and breakdown are dissimilar from this book in that there are specific offenders excluded from this consideration, by design. Hickey correctly notes that some

6 Real-Life Monsters

serial killer traits can be eliminated from the definition due to “speculation” rather “than verifiable evidence given the current state of serial murder re- search” (1997, p. 12). The direction of this book is to consider a psychologi- cal theory originally formulated by personal speculation, and then to evaluate its merit in the context of verified evidence.

Steven Egger’s (1990, p. 4) definition of a serial killer states:

a serial murder occurs when one or more individuals (males, in most known cases) commit a second or subsequent murder; is relation- shipless; is at a different time and has no apparent connection to the initial murder; and is usually committed in a different geographical loca- tion. Further, the motive is not for material gain and is believed to be for the murderer’s desire to have power over the victims.

Egger’s definition also includes a specific consideration of the victims, of- fering that they may have a symbolic value and insinuating that they are se- lected often for their perceived vulnerability. He further defines this in his 2002 book, The Killers Among Us , where he explains victims typically “in- clude vagrants, the homeless, prostitutes, migrant workers, homosexuals, missing children, single women, elderly women, college students and hospital patients” (p. 5). The assumptions regarding the serial murderer definition in this book will more closely resemble Egger’s than any other, differing mainly in reduced consideration of victim status and concentration on the killer’s personality, internal motivators, and development. While a certain vulnerable victim is a clear and favorite serial killer target, the sexual interest target is also a substantial one.

The focus here will be on what would appear to be the essence of the phenomenon of this particular criminal. The definition of a serial murderer, which parameters this book will be operating under, will therefore be narrow; much more narrow than most studies of the subject. There will be an attempt to identify a very specific psychological dynamic in theory. Mass murderers and spree killers, for example, are not included, as they appear to involve an entirely different set of psychodynamics.

AREN’T THEY ALL MASS MURDERERS? This is a critical part of this book. It’s routine for the newspapers to refer

to serial killers, spree killers, and mass murderers all in interchangeable terms. However, the focus of this book is to correctly and definitely identify the psychological makeup of the serial killer. Why should we care? That will be discussed in the Conclusion.

The fact is there are very different sets of dynamics at work in each type of offender. While a mass murderer in general language is a person who kills a group of people, in more specific terms he or she is the offender who purpose- fully kills a large group at a single event. George Hennard, who in 1991 drove

Introduction: An Identifi cation of the Offender 7

a truck into a Luby’s restaurant in Killeen, Texas, and then executed as many people as possible with a handgun, is a very different psychological case than a calculated cold-blooded predator like Anthony Sowell. The prototypical mass-murdering school shooters or workplace massacres or a violent rampage by a jilted spouse, such as that of Robert Stewart who shot eight elderly nurs- ing home residents in North Carolina in 2009, bear no resemblance in their very public acts as compared to a long-term patient predator who kills many carefully selected victims over time. A spree killer such as Andrew Cunanan leaving a trail of seemingly random victims behind represents an entirely dif- ferent mind-set than the young photography targets of Rodney Alcala.

Eliminated are murderers of a serial nature who have any financial or other tangible motivation that might render incidental the killing aspect of the crime. Also eliminated from the study is the cult-obsessed type, as the true serial killer—or at least the type this analysis tries to identify—may be drawn to cult activities, but not necessarily. The serial murderer kills because he or she wants to, not because he or she is mindlessly led by a cult. This targeted personality may carry out killings in a serial manner owing to an association with a cult, but not because he or she is merely guided or even controlled by that influence. The specific psychological phenomenon to be identified here is a development of internal factors, owing someone to habitually kill for the implicit thrill, satisfaction, or satiation of the act; or as Herman Mudgett put it: for the “pleasure of killing my fellow beings” (Editors of Time-Life Books, 1992, p. 95). The ultimate control of another human being and the accompanying catharsis are the psychological hallmarks of the individuals to be discussed.

The reader must understand that the focus of this book is to try to identify and characterize the individual who kills for the joy of killing. Unlike Hickey’s broader population, this book will consider a smaller, more focused sample set of data, defined by greater consideration of preliminary information. The examination will be of a group of offenders whose need and motivations to kill appear unencumbered by extraneous variables, such as money or outside influence.

THE PROBLEM OF ESTIMATING NUMBERS The actual prevalence of this type of offender is difficult to pinpoint. His-

torical estimates have ranged from 6,000+ victims a year (McKay, 1985), to 4,000 a year (Lindsey, 1984), to 3,500 to 5,000 a year (Holmes & DeBurger, 1988). Kenna Kiger noted that active offenders have been estimated as low as 30 to as high as 500, depending on the source of research (Egger, 1990, p. 37). Egger later referenced a disagreement between the FBI and the CNN, who reduced the former’s estimate of serial killers between 1977 and 1992 from 331 to 175 after eliminating duplications. An FBI agent quoted in the CNN report guessed at “25, 35, or 40” active at a time, whereas another guessed probably 50–100 (Egger, 2002, p. 68). These figures represent activity in the

8 Real-Life Monsters

United States, as cross-cultural research is extremely limited. There is little agreement, and very little in the way of reliable accuracy in numbers, other than the fact that these killers exist and continue to thrive in our society. Even the aforementioned Texas symposium published in 2011 didn’t attempt to rigidly place a number beyond the fact that serial murder is relatively rare in the grand scheme of murder and that there is a consensus that many suc- cessful killers have gone undetected (Hargrove, 2011). The FBI stated that approximately 2 percent of homicides reported to Violent Criminal Appre- hension Program (ViCAP), on file with the bureau in Quantico, Virginia, are attributed to serial murder. Retired FBI profiler Mark Safarik guessed there were up to two dozen serial killers operating now.

A 2010 USA Today report stated, “during the past four decades, at least 459 people may have died at the hands of highway serial killers, FBI statistics show.” Suspects included as many as 200 long-haul truck drivers, with at least 10 suspects believed to be involved in 30 or more murders. However, the report went on to state it was very difficult to pin down actual serial killings as opposed to other types of crime, including drug trade. Figuring out how old a particular crime is also is challenging. Many victims included prostitutes, a favorite disposable target of serial killers. A highway rest stop would seem to be an ideal spot for serial killers to operate, with prostitutes engaging in high-risk activity and the highway offering an easy-access escape route. Infor- mation continues to be fed to the FBI Highway Serial Killings database, but sorting out the results remains unclear. Still, an agent who oversaw the FBI effort for three years stated, “We seem to have one a week that comes in. . . . They’re out there” (Morrison, 2010, pp. 1, 2A).

In 2011, long-haul trucker John Boyer pleaded guilty to the murder of a woman in North Carolina and confessed, without a hint of remorse, to the killings of women in Tennessee and South Carolina, and was suspected in more (Collins & Dalesio, 2011). Investigators were taken aback by the unbri- dled hatred for women by the man who lived with his mother near Augusta, Georgia. One investigator was greeted in an interview with Boyer by, “What bitch are you here about?”

The media have called attention to the activities of law enforcement agen- cies in apprehending the perpetrators of these sensational crimes. As well, the direct relationship between the media and serial killers is clear. Gibson (2006) concludes that in all cases of serial killing the “interrelationships between se- rial killers, law enforcement, the media and the public are complex,” media coverage influences the killers as well as the investigation, and “serial killers and serial murder cases influence the media” (p. xvii).

Undeniably, these issues are of critical importance and relevance. However, the direction and perspective of this book is psychological, putting forth a blueprint of the development and motivation of the serial killer. Discussed here are some of the current theories commonly applied to this phenomenon. These theories are then combined with admitted intuition and speculative

Introduction: An Identifi cation of the Offender 9

insight to develop and consider a theoretical model of the psychopathology of the serial killer.

VICTIMOLOGY Another key element is, when trying to identify a serial murderer, that

there must be a presence of a certain link to the victims. Victims of a mass murder generally have no connection other than being in the same place (the wrong place) at the same time, such as the victims at Columbine, Richard Speck, Luby’s, or Virginia Tech. A serial murderer, the type that this book attempts to identify as a psychological personality type, generally has a rhyme or reason to his or her victim choice—whether it’s physical, pretty young girls with hair parted in the middle for Bundy, or a general sexual target as with Gacy or Dahmer, or disposable sexual surrogates as with Shawcross, or simply a person engaging in a high-risk personal lifestyle who is an easy target that might not be missed (victims referred to as the “less dead” by Steven Egger), victims of killers like Dahmer, Shawcross, and Anthony Sowell. Generally, a thread that ties the victim pool together can be realized when identifying a serial murderer. Some may be carefully stalked, while others could be simply unlucky, found in the wrong place at the worst possible moment, as in the cases of the Zodiac Killer, Gary Ridgway, and David Berkowitz. Victimology, along with psychological state, is usually how a serial murderer is separated from a mass murderer or a spree killer, and is a critical point within the profile assembled of a particular offender.

DO WE KNOW WHAT WE THINK WE KNOW? Finally, the study of serial murder continues to humble even the most ex-

perienced researcher. I was told by Dr. Steven Egger, as renowned an expert on the subject as there is, that the more one studies this subject, the more comes the realization of how little one knows (personal communication, 1993). Competing theory and additional knowledge continue to confound and blur a topic we would all hope could be much more clear.

In 2003, I clipped an article in the London Guardian about Lucy de Berk, an accused serial killer nurse from the Netherlands (Osborn, 2003). The case was interesting because not only did she appear to kill at least seven patients in an angel of mercy fashion (and as well was suspected in many more), she fit into many serial killer profiles, both in gender and in behavior. Stories of her “compulsions” from her diary fit neatly into commonly held psychological ideas regarding this type of offender’s profile. The FBI published an article in the Law Enforcement Bulletin (Brantley & Kosky, 2005) in 2005, analyzing de Berk’s case in terms of motivation, behavior, and characteristics. In the context of serial murder in general, Lucy’s crimes were viewed as not atypical, especially within the realm of “heath-care worker serial murderers.”

10 Real-Life Monsters

Lucy de Berk’s patients at the hospital in the Hague seemed to unexpect- edly die. Suddenly. A hospital review determined that not only had 8 patients under her care died, but 19 elderly patients had died while under her care in a previous facility. Another died at home after a visit from Lucy. She was ar- rested for the murder of 13 patients in 2001.

In 2002, the District Attorney’s office and the Hague Police Department presented the case to the FBI’s BAU at the National Center for the Analysis of Violent Crime. An on-site review resulted in the eventual expert testimony of BAU personnel regarding the motivation, behavior, and psychological characteristics of serial murderers (Brantley & Kosky, 2005). It was noted that female serial murderers kill those in close proximity, including those within custodial care as in the cases of health care serial murderers. Issues including power, recognition, excitement as well as a reduction of stress and tension were discussed as reasons for health care workers, primarily female, to kill.

In Lucy de Berk’s case, she repeatedly wrote in her diary about giving in to her compulsions. She wrestled with confusion over her lack of feelings or compassion. She strove to understand her self-described “sociopathic person- ality” (Brantley & Kosky, 2005). She stated, “I don’t even know why I am doing it. . . . I will take this secret with me to the grave. . . . Still, I hope I am helping people by this” (Osborn, 2003, p. 13).

The FBI reported de Berk had established a close relationship in the case of every victim and had been seen in the vicinity of them before their deaths. Oddly, Lucy reported deterioration in the patients, which contradicted other nurses’ observations. She had been accused of stealing drugs that happened to be the same ones the victims had been taking. Intoxication of specific drugs was noted in the deaths of at least two little girls.

Lucy was convicted of four counts of first-degree murder for the injection deaths of three babies and an elderly woman and three counts of attempted murder as well as perjury, falsifying a high school diploma, and theft of several books from the prison library. She received life imprisonment. On appeal, she was convicted of seven counts of murder and four counts of attempted murder.

However, after serving six years of a life sentence, de Berk was not only cleared, released, and exonerated of the crimes, she received a personal apol- ogy by the country’s attorney general Harm Brouwer, and a financial set- tlement was discussed (“Court Clears,” 2010). It was determined during a subsequent retrial that much of the evidence, highly speculative statistics of probability, was faulty and other circumstantial evidence invalid.

An assertion by a statistician that the odds of her being present during each suspicious death were 1 in 342 million was debunked. A witness who testified about hearing incriminating statements recanted. Deaths previously ruled suspicious were now deemed not to have signs of being unnatural. The damning words found in de Berk’s diary regarding her compulsions were later explained away as her desire to use Tarot cards, or according to de Berk’s daughter were “pure fiction” written for a novel.

Introduction: An Identifi cation of the Offender 11

Were there serial murders committed in those hospitals in the Netherlands? It is simply impossible to conclude. It is also impossible to ascertain the in- volvement of anyone at that scene at this point in time. Still, the de Berk case is an example that facts and analysis of both concluded cases and theoretical concepts accepted for years may or may not be accurate, and may continue to evolve.

This page intentionally left blank

Clinical Diagnoses and Serial Killer Traits

2

I actually think I may be possessed with demons, I was dropped on my head as a kid.

—Dennis Rader/BTK

CLINICAL PHENOMENA AND THE SERIAL KILLER Many syndromes and clusters of personality traits as recognized by the psycho- logical community are noted in the literature regarding serial killers. The fol- lowing labels and diagnoses are often assigned to these offenders at some point in their contact with psychologists or psychiatrists, both before and after their crimes. Also, many law enforcement agencies, in studying this type of offender, point to the following characteristics and clinical phenomena in an effort to un- derstand the thought and behavior patterns driving these offenders’ behaviors.

THE ANTISOCIAL PERSONALITY Often, serial killers are described as psychopaths or sociopaths, or in more

widely accepted terminology, as having dissocial or antisocial personality dis- order (APD). According to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual, Fourth Edition Text Revision ( DSM-IV-TR ) (American Psychiatric Association [APA], 2000, p. 706), the criteria for this disorder includes:

A. Pervasive pattern of disregard for and violation of the rights of others, occurring since age 15 as indicated by at least three of the following:

14 Real-Life Monsters

1. Failure to conform to social norms with respect to lawful behav- ior as indicated by repeatedly performing acts that are grounds for arrest.

2. Deceitfulness, as indicated by repeated lying, use of aliases, or conning others for personal profi t or pleasure.

3. Impulsivity or failure to plan ahead. 4. Irritability and aggressiveness, as indicated by repeated physicals

fi ghts or assaults. 5. Reckless disregard for the safety of self or others. 6. Consistent irresponsibility, as indicated by repeated failure to

sustain consistent work behavior or honor fi nancial obligations. 7. Lack of remorse, as indicated by indifference to or rationalizing

having hurt, mistreated, or stolen from another. B. Individual is at least 18 years of age. C. Evidence of conduct disorder onset before age 15. D. The occurrence of the behavior is not exclusively during the course

of schizophrenia or a manic episode.

These criteria are particularly relevant to the serial killer, as many such offenders share childhoods colored with demonstrations of behavior con- sistent with criteria for conduct disorder, such as using weapons, physical cruelty to people and animals, forced sexual activity, and fire setting ( APA, 2000 pp. 98–99). Key elements shared by the sociopath or the antisocial per- sonality, and the serial killer are a failure to conform to social norms regarding lawful behavior, physical aggressiveness, impulsivity, lack of regard for the truth, manipulativeness, and most important, lack of remorse or empathy. Remorse is reported by some serial killers after commission of some crimes, but never to the point of their changing their behavior or seeking help, such as Dodd, Nilsen, Dahmer, Bundy, and others.

Causal factors for an antisocial personality include a possible biological pre- disposition (Andreasen, 1984), childhood trauma (shared by the vast major- ity of serial killers), possible neurological factors in the control of impulsivity regarding serotonin levels in the brain, and heredity. The DSM-IV-TR states that APD is more common among the first-degree biological relatives of those with the disorder than among the general population. Adoption studies indicate that both genetic and environmental factors contribute to the risk (APA, 2000, p. 704).

Those diagnosed with antisocial personalities also share deep-seated doubts regarding their own adequacy (Havens, personal communication, 1993). Most antisocial personalities are men, again reflecting serial killer demographics.

It should be noted, however, that it is not enough to simply describe the serial killer as an antisocial personality. The vast majority of the current prison population shares this diagnosis, as well as many average citizens (Politicians? Car salesmen?) who are not incarcerated. Actually, most of us at some time or

Clinical Diagnoses and Serial Killer Traits 15

another could look over the DSM-IV-TR’s criteria and recognize a few quali- ties close to home. This clustering of traits is helpful, however, as it adds to an overall profile and suggests possible consistencies and etiologies, but the criteria should not be overrated.

THE PSYCHOPATHIC PERSONALITY Antisocial, sociopath, and psychopath are not interchangeable terms, as

often commonly used. Psychopath is a far more severe psychological condi- tion in terms of symptoms and treatment. According to Blair, Mitchell, and Blair while 80 percent of U.S. inmates reach diagnostic criteria for APD, only 15–25 percent meet the criteria for psychopathy as established by Hare (Blair et al., 2005, p. 19). Psychopaths and sociopaths are not separately listed in the DSM-IV-TR, nor are they found in the World Health Orga- nization’s ICD-10 classification. Psychopathy was a term used in earlier versions of the DSM but gradually fell under the label of antisocial and is considered basically a synonym of antisocial, describing a subject with a lack of empathy and attachment to others while exhibiting manipulative behavior.

The ICD-10 classification of dissocial identity disorder may be meant to mirror the APD listing, but it does mention a little more in the way of behav- ior, namely that the behavior is not “readily modifiable by adverse experience, including punishment” and a “low threshold for discharge of aggression, including violence,” both characteristics sounding closer to a psychopathic personality. Personality traits listed include amoral, antisocial, asocial, psycho- pathic, and sociopathic (World Health Organization, 2008, p. 345).

This book, however, strongly supports the position that the psychopaths (and sociopaths) are markedly more severe offenders than antisocials, as es- tablished by research on their behaviors. Reportedly the DSM-V, currently under development, will re-examine the character issues that separate them from mere antisocials.

The psychopathic offender appears to be predisposed for predatory vio- lence and is the classic serial killer personality. Indeed, this type of personal- ity was the subject of J. Reid Meloy’s book, The Psychopathic Mind: Origins, Dynamics, & Treatment (1988).

The psychopath, according to Meloy, is a personality incorporating both aggressive narcissism and extended chronic antisocial behavior over time. The personal history of psychopaths shows a trail of used, injured, and hurt people as these individuals tarnish their object world in a continuous effort to build their own fragile sense of self. Robert Hare (1993) noted that psycho- paths “have little aptitude for experiencing the emotional responses—fear and anxiety—that are the mainsprings of conscience” (p. 76).

In identifying the psychopath, the revised Hare Psychopathy Checklist (PCL-R :2nd ed.) (Hare, 2003) measures such traits, most if not all of which are found in the serial killer personality:

16 Real-Life Monsters

1. glibness and superfi cial charm 2. grandiosity 3. continuous need for stimulation 4. pathological lying 5. conning and manipulativeness 6. lack of remorse or guilt 7. shallow affect 8. callous lack of empathy 9. parasitic lifestyle 10. poor behavioral controls 11. promiscuity 12. early behavior problems 13. lack of realistic, long-term goals 14. impulsivity 15. irresponsibility 16. failure to accept responsibility for actions 17. many short-term relationships 18. juvenile delinquency 19. revocation of conditional release 20. criminal versatility

These traits are comparable to those traits found in the antisocial personal- ity, but the checklist goes a step further in identifying internal as well as ex- ternal characteristics. This clustering is similar to ones observed in Cleckley’s classic work Mask of Sanity (1941).

The defining trait of the psychopath has to be considered his or her com- plete lack of empathy, to the point of simply not understanding remorse or the concern for others. He or she may be able to feign remorse and attempt to say the right things, usually through repetition of counseling and time in the system. In 1989, just hours before his execution, Ted Bundy was inter- viewed by Dr. James Dobson in a video shot on Death Row in Florida entitled Fatal Addiction . Much of Bundy’s talk attempted to underline a connection between his development as a killer and an exposure to violent pornography. However, when specifically asked about remorse, his rambling three-and-a- half minute answer sounded like the classic psychopath who was grasping for the words to sound like someone who could convince others of his regret. His words were unconvincingly hollow:

D OBSON : [I]s the remorse . . . there? B UNDY : I know that people will accuse me of being self-serving but

we’re beyond that now, I mean I’m just telling you how I feel . . . but, through God’s help, I’ve been able to come to the point where I’ve, much too late, but better late than never . . . feel the hurt and the pain I’ve been responsible for. Yes, absolutely. In the past few days myself and a number of investigators have been talking about unsolved cases.

Clinical Diagnoses and Serial Killer Traits 17

Bundy went straight from trying to convince the listener he was remorse- ful for his murders to essentially selling his value as someone on death row who could help clear unsolved cases, to discussing violence in the media, people with predisposition like his, kids watching TV at home, and could not help but come across as a person who could not understand the concept of remorse beyond a detached attempt at saying what he thought people might want to hear.

During the penalty phase of his trial, Anthony Sowell took the stand and apologized to his victims’ families, without much explanation. After he was given the death penalty, a juror later stated, “we found it [his statement] to be completely rehearsed, completely condescending, complete lack of remorse.” Another juror reported Sowell winked at her at one point (“Sowell jury: death sentence,” 2011). A surviving victim who saw a headless corpse in Sowell’s house said, “Does he feel shame? Does he feel anything?” (Seewer & Sheeran, 2011).

The psychopath is more frequently and severely violent than the antisocial personality, and the violence continues until it reaches a plateau at age 50 or so, whereas nonviolent activity drops off sooner (Hare, McPherson, & Forth, 1988). The psychopath is generally regarded as untreatable.

A key aspect of the psychopath in regard to serial killers is that the violence tends to be predatory and primarily on a stranger-to-stranger basis. The vio- lence is planned, purposeful, and emotionless. This emotionlessness reflects a detached, fearless, and possibly dissociated state, revealing a lower level of impulses generated by the autonomic nervous system and a lack of anxiety. The psychopath’s general motivation is to control and dominate, and his his- tory reveals no bonds with others.

Sexually, psychopaths continue their grandiose demeanor and are hy- poaroused autonomically, which causes them to be continuously seeking sensation. Their attitude is one of entitlement, not reciprocity. This lack of bonding reflects a lack of emotionality and a diminished capacity for love, where sexual partners are partly objects and are devalued. The psychopath also displays a propensity for sadism (Meloy, 1993). Stanton Samenow once noted that the criminal experiences an “adrenaline rush” by “fantasizing about vio- lence, talking about violence, and behaving violently” (2004, p. 102).

It should be noted that psychopaths tend to “engage in more instrumen- tal, goal-driven homicides,” whereas “non-pychopathic offenders engage in predominantly reactive, spontaneous violence” (Bartol & Bartol, 2008, p. 271). Regardless of the psychopath’s personal goal and lack of empathy, his behavior is planned and purposeful as opposed to emotional and in response to a stimulus.

In 2010, the journal Psychological Science noted a study by University of New Mexico psychologists who observed that psychopaths know the differ- ence between right and wrong, but can fail to weigh the difference when making decisions (Monteleone, 2010). It was observed that the psychopathic inmates studied had demonstrated difficulty in making inferences connecting

18 Real-Life Monsters

negative consequences to risky situations as well as failing to connect required actions or compensations to behaviors an average person would understand, such as putting gas in a car after borrowing it.

Experiments conducted by researchers at the University of Wisconsin-Mad- ison (UW) demonstrated psychopaths behaving in a manner similar to those suffering brain damage, reported in the journal Neuropsychologia (Doherty, 2010). The experiments support the theory that a defect or wiring flaw in a portion of the brain behind the eyes called the ventromedial prefrontal cortex is responsible for psychopathic behavior. This part of the brain has been identified as the origin for emotions like empathy, guilt, and shame. Research involving games played by inmates at the Wisconsin Department of Corrections showed prisoners diagnosed with primary psychopathy who played the games with UW researchers used the same kinds of strategies and made similar decisions to play- ers who had suffered devastating damage, often due to medical conditions such as strokes and tumors, to this portion of their brain. These subjects reportedly had led normal emotional lives until their brains were damaged.

Blair et al. concluded that if an individual’s neurocognitive systems in- volved in the regulation of reactive aggression and emotional learning are impaired at an early age, the individual “will present with the emotional dif- ficulties associated with psychopathy.” Affected areas will include socialization that could result in “elevated levels of instrumental aggression” (Blair et al., 2005, p. 141).

Again, it should also be noted, that the psychopath terminology is com- monly used interchangeably with the possibly more commonly used term sociopath . In the purest of technical considerations, a sociopath is an offender who more likely learned his or her behavior, while the psychopath appears born with the personality. In any case both offenders will look very much alike in the ensuing discussions of this book: marked absence of empathy, manipulative nature, and using other people, an aggressive, predatory pattern of behavior and no regard for consequences.

Both will also display a certain immaturity that seems to go with the psy- chopath or the sociopath, a dimension of the personality seeming to reflect a holding pattern at a particular age of development, which almost explains the narcissistic, selfish, and uncaring perspective. Dr. Betty McMahon described Danny Rolling as “extremely immature . . . there is a great impairment of em- pathy. That is something that tends to come with maturity” (Ryzuk, 1994, p. 388). Rolling said once that “something died” at a very young age and he would always be immature and inadequate (Rolling & London, 1996, p. 9).

BORDERLINE PERSONALITY DISORDER A comparable condition to APD is borderline personality disorder, which

includes a pervasive pattern of instability in interpersonal relationships, self- image, affects, and marked impulsivity. This pattern begins by early adulthood and is present in a variety of contexts, as indicated by the presence of at least five of the following ( APA, 2000, pp. 706–710):

Clinical Diagnoses and Serial Killer Traits 19

1. Frantic efforts to avoid real or imagined abandonment. 2. A pattern of unstable and intense interpersonal relationships charac-

terized by alternating extremes of idealization and devaluation. 3. Identity disturbance; markedly and persistently unstable self-image

or sense of self. 4. Impulsivity in at least two areas that are potentially self-damaging. 5. Recurrent suicidal behavior, gestures, threats, or self-mutilating

behavior. 6. Affective (emotional) instability due to a marked reactivity of mood. 7. Chronic feelings of emptiness. 8. Inappropriate, intense anger or diffi culty controlling anger. 9. Transient, stress-related paranoid ideation or severe dissociative

symptoms.

Again, this disorder includes a defective sense of identity and extreme in- stability. The sufferer often views the world and people as “all good” or “all bad” (Baron-Cohen, 2011, p. 55). This description could relate to the large percentage of female serial killers who act as angels of mercy, who attempt to right the world’s wrongs, or who seek revenge and owe the world (or some part of it) a payback. Aileen Wuornos was diagnosed by a prison neuropsy- chologist as meeting all eight of the criteria for borderline personality disorder (Reynolds, 1992).

Although borderline personality disorder is more often (75%) diagnosed in females, it is not exclusively so, as even Jeffrey Dahmer was diagnosed by prison psychiatrists as having features of this disorder (Dvorchak & Holewa, 1991). Arthur Shawcross also exhibited such characteristics, and John Wayne Gacy was described by a forensic psychiatrist, Richard Rappaport, as utiliz- ing borderline personality organization. Rappaport stated that Gacy brought young boys to his home so he could “star in a play scripted by himself,” illus- trating a primitive ego defense common to borderlines: projective identifica- tion (Cahill, 1986, pp. 339–340).

Causal factors in borderline personality disorder include a history of incest or other sexual abuse and a proneness to experience dysphoria, or a general- ized feeling of ill-being, as well as abnormal anxiety, discontent, or physical discomfort, commonly thought to be connected to a problem of the limbic system. Dissociative symptoms may occur during extreme stress. Borderline is approximately five times more common among first-degree biological rela- tives with the disorder than in the general population.

Both APD and borderline personality disorder display gross deviation from normal attachment processes, which results in a disinhibition of violence (Meloy, 1993). The borderline individual will be pathologically attached, whereas at the other end of the spectrum is the antisocial, who is patho- logically detached. Pathological attachment development issues often result in personalities that are criminal, and exhibit traits of the sociopath or the psychopath.

20 Real-Life Monsters

DISSOCIATION Another phenomenon usually considered in the psychology of the serial

murderer is the dissociative state or disorder. Dissociation (Egger, 1990) is the lack of integration of thoughts, feelings, and experiences into the stream of consciousness. In other words, it is a mental separation from the physical place of an individual. Dissociation has been cited as an example of spontane- ous self-hypnosis (Bliss, 1986, p. 166). The phenomenon has been used to describe peoples’ reactions to various traumatic experiences, as well as a pre- cursor to pathologies described in the DSM-IV-TR, such as fugues, amnesias, depersonalization, dissociative identity disorder (formerly multiple personal- ity disorder), and posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD).

The DSM-IV-TR (2000) describes general dissociative disorders as a dis- ruption in the usually integrated functions of consciousness, memory, iden- tity, or perception (p. 519). Disorders include depersonalization disorder, with criteria such as experiencing a feeling of detachment from, and as if one is an outside observer of, one’s mental processes or body, like one is in a dream. This statement has been made by many serial murderers.

Morton Prince (1975, p. 291) referred to dissociative states as:

a large category of conditions characterized by alteration of the per- sonality. In this category are to be found various types of alteration, some normal and some abnormal, all due to the same processes and mechanisms and therefore fundamentally resembling one another, in that they are all types of depersonalization and repersonalization from the standpoint of the modern conception of the structure of the per- sonality. Specifically, these types are known as sleep, hypnosis, fugues, trance, somnambulisms, multiple personality, etc.

Causality regarding dissociative states includes severe childhood trauma and some evidence of a physical predisposition. Many of those who experi- ence dissociative states are of above-average intelligence, another trait found in many serial killers.

Literature involving serial killers and the possible presence of a dissociative state is extensive:

Jeffrey Dahmer : He couldn’t embrace. He couldn’t touch. His eyes were dead.

(Dvorchak & Holewa, 1991, p. 32)

Ted Bundy : I looked up at Ted and our eyes locked. His face had gone blank, as though he was not there at all.

(Kendall, 1981)

Clinical Diagnoses and Serial Killer Traits 21

Bundy himself stated after a murder he would be coming out of a hor- rible trance or dream.

(Dobson, 1989)

Dayton Leroy Rogers : He seemed to be slipping in and out of a fantasy state [while calling the victim someone else’s name] . . . he was all- consumed by the deep mental state he was in.

(King, 1992, pp. 30, 38)

Wayne Nance : He [the victim] looked him straight in the eye. He saw nothing: no glee, no remorse, just a dead gaze.

(Coston, 1992, p. 313)

Bobby Joe Long : It was like a dream me doing it.

(Norris, 1992, p. 125)

Even beyond the serial killer, violent crime continues reports of what would seem to be dissociative states. An Indiana teenager who strangled his 10-year-old brother told his girlfriend he wanted to be just like a fictional TV serial killer and fantasized about killing people since the eighth grade. Andrew Conley described the murder as “watching the murder from outside himself” (Wilson, 2010). A Jamaican American mass murderer on Long Island, Colin Ferguson shot and killed 6 and wounded 19 more while being described as “he had a blank look on his face” (Ramsland, 2005a). In Kansas City, a woman convicted of killing an expectant mother, cutting the baby from her womb and stealing the child, reported being in a dreamlike state during the murder (Duclos, 2008). In 2011, Levi Aron admitted to killing an eight-year- old and dismembering him. Later, he was described in a psychiatric evaluation as, “his mood is neutral, practically blank” (“Levi Aron,” 2011).

It should be noted that it’s not uncommon for defense attorneys to con- coct insanity defenses centered around offenders who don’t remember com- mitting an offense. Dissociative states should not be confused with psychotic or delusional episodes or hallucinations. Still, many of these reports of disas- sociation are plausible especially in the cases of serial killers and others when the reports are made by witnesses’ observations.

These episodes indicate a certain level of dissociative process, albeit on a minor scale. Usually, the process does not appear as a full-blown dissociative disorder, such as a psychogenic fugue state or multiple personality disorder and does not enter into the psychopathology of the serial killer. These disor- ders have not been documented or confirmed with any frequency (if at all) and are often the basis for an attempt at malingering or are used as the basis for an insanity defense (e.g., Bianchi, Gacy).

22 Real-Life Monsters

Drawing a parallel between a psychopathic personality and the dissociated demeanor of the serial murderer, Meloy (1992) noted that “psychopathy is, among other things, a disorder of profound detachment.” He added, “from this conscienceless, detached psychology emerges a heightened risk of vio- lence, most notably a capacity for predation” (p. xvii).

DOUBLING The discussion of dissociation leads to mention of a recent theory in the

psychology of rationalizing killing, that of doubling. Robert J. Lifton, in The Nazi Doctors (2000, p. 418), refers to doubling as:

the division of the self into two functioning wholes, so that a part-self acts as an entire self. An Auschwitz doctor could, through doubling, not only kill and contribute to killing but organize silently, on behalf of that evil project, an entire self-structure (or self process) encompassing virtually all aspects of his behavior.

This self process could easily be the phenomenon exhibited when a killer appears to be in a different or detached state, watching what is going on rather than being the direct participant, thus removing himself or herself from the feelings and responsibilities of murder. Lifton speaks of the benefits of doubling, including the connection of the two selves. This connection could allow serial killers to put on a mask of sanity when not participating in crimes, as well as to avoid guilt, which might otherwise be utilized in a typical anti- social personality. Finally, the unconscious dimension of doubling takes place largely outside of awareness, allowing an alteration of moral consciousness. The serial killer who attaches critical importance to his or her acts, and is driven by the fantasy and then the act, can incorporate those acts through this unconscious dimension. It is an active psychological process, a means of adaptation to extremity (pp. 418–430). Robert Ressler, veteran profiler for the FBI, has stated “psychopaths . . . are known for their ability to separate the personality who commits the crimes from their more in-control selves” (1992, p. 154). This sounds very much like Lifton’s principles.

James S. Grotstein speaks of the development of a “separate being, living within one that has been preconsciously split off and has an independent ex- istence with independent motivation, separate agenda, etc.” and from which can emanate “evil, sadism, destructiveness or even demoniacal possession” (1979, pp. 36–52). He attributes its development to those elements of the self that have been artificially suppressed and disavowed early in life.

The phenomenon of doubling appears to have been observed even by Freud (1938), who coined the term splitting to identify dissociation in re- lation to repression. This was further specified by Kohut (1971, pp. 176– 177, 183) by conceptualizing vertical, rather than horizontal, splits in the psyche, noting the “side-by-side existence of cohesive personality attitudes

Clinical Diagnoses and Serial Killer Traits 23

with different goal structures, different pleasure aims, different moral and aesthetic values.”

According to Lifton, doubling can include elements considered character- istic of sociopathic impairment, such as a disorder of feelings, pathological avoidance of a sense of guilt, and resort to violence to overcome a masked depression. Murderous behavior may thereby cover a feared disintegration of the self, a concept that appears so critical and so damaged in the view of a serial killer.

NARCISSISTIC PERSONALITY DISORDER The DSM-IV-TR describes the narcissistic personality as that having a per-

vasive pattern of grandiosity (in fantasy or behavior), with need for admira- tion and a lack of empathy. The pattern begins by early adulthood, and is present in a variety of contexts, as indicated by at least five of the following (2000, pp. 714–717):

1. Has a grandiose sense of self-importance. 2. Is preoccupied with fantasies of unlimited success, power, brilliance,

beauty, or ideal love. 3. Believes that he or she is special and unique and can only be under-

stood by, or should associate with, other special or high-status peo- ple or institutions.

4. Requires excessive admiration. 5. Has a sense of entitlement. 6. Is interpersonally exploitative. 7. Lacks empathy. 8. Is often envious of others or believes that others are envious of him

or her. 9. Shows arrogant, haughty behaviors or attitudes.

Most of these traits can be found in the serial killer’s personality (e.g., Bundy, Wuornos, Alcala, Gacy—and so many more). Aggressive narcissism is pervasive in the classic psychopath and features a pronounced sadistic streak (Meloy, 1992).

All these features were found in Angelo Buono and Kenneth Bianchi, the “Hillside Stranglers.” When asked by a prison inmate why he killed all those girls, Buono brazenly declared, “They were no good, they deserved to die. It had to be done” (O’Brien, 1985, p. 301). Many other witnesses, including even Bianchi, noted one of Buono’s favorite phrases was “some girls deserve to die.”

OBSESSIVE COMPULSIVENESS Another pattern that seems to emerge with serial killers is the presence

of obsessive-compulsive traits. Obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) can

24 Real-Life Monsters

manifest in obsessions, defined as recurrent and persistent ideas, thoughts, im- pulses, or images, that are experienced as intrusive and inappropriate and that cause anxiety or distress—for example, a parent’s horrific impulses to hurt one’s child ( APA, 2000, p. 457). Additionally, the thoughts, impulses, or im- ages are not simply excessive worries about real-life problems; the person at- tempts to ignore or suppress such thoughts or impulses or to neutralize them with other thoughts or actions. The person, while owning a sense that the content of the obsession is not the kind of thought he or she would expect to have, recognizes these obsessions as a product of his or her own mind (p. 457).

Also apparent are compulsions, defined as repetitive behaviors or mental acts performed in order to prevent or reduce anxiety or distress. The behav- ior is designed to reduce the distress that accompanies an obsession or to prevent some dreaded event or situation; however, the activity is not con- nected in a realistic way with what it is designed to neutralize or it is clearly excessive. Also, the person recognizes that his or her behavior is excessive or unreasonable. In the course of the disorder after repeated failure to resist the obsessions and compulsions, the individual may give in to them, no longer experience a desire to resist them and may incorporate them into his or her daily routines ( APA, 2000, pp. 457–458).

Similar behavior patterns on a smaller scale (obsessive-compulsive person- ality disorder) feature a number of diagnostic criteria, four of which are re- quired for diagnosis of the disorder. They include:

1. Preoccupation with details, rules, lists, order, organization, or sched- ules to the extent that the major point of the activity is lost.

2. Shows perfectionism that interferes with task completion. 3. Is excessively devoted to work and productivity to the exclusion of

leisure activities and friendships (not accounted for by obvious eco- nomic necessity).

4. Is overconscientious, scrupulous, and infl exible about matters of morality, ethics, or values.

5. Is unable to discard worn-out or worthless objects even when they have no sentimental value.

6. Is reluctant to delegate tasks or to work with others unless they sub- mit to exactly his or her way of doing things.

7. Adopts a miserly spending style toward both self and others; money is viewed as something to be hoarded for future catastrophes.

8. Shows rigidity and stubbornness ( APA, 2000, p. 729).

Obsessive-compulsive people often have problems expressing aggressive feelings and so they stifle them, causing an implosion of emotions that could cause great internal damage. They often have a history of stress, are twice as often males, and are often children of people with OCD or personality disor- der themselves. The obsessive-compulsive condition often precedes the onset of depression.

Clinical Diagnoses and Serial Killer Traits 25

Also apparent is a biological link other than the aforementioned heredity. One hypothesis is there are communication difficulties between the brain’s frontal lobes and its basal ganglia, buried deep in the lower part of the brain. This creates problems of integrating sensory, motor, and cognitive processes, and results in persistent unwanted thoughts and involuntary actions (Bruno, 1993, p. 147).

It seems that these traits are yet another dimension easily applied to a great number of serial killers, yet often overlooked by the literature. Consider the obsessive rituals of a David Berkowitz, the compulsive habits of a Day- ton Rogers or Robert Berdella, the terror of the dreaded event (potentially being alone) of a Jeffrey Dahmer. The obsessive compulsion of killing is easily viewed as a strategy to avoid the distress in store after failing to find a victim when the urge is great. Obsessive-compulsive behavior often shows up in males, and there are strong suggestions of genetic or biological links. Most important, the extreme insistence of these personalities that others follow their rules sounds deadly similar to the emotions of those whose main pathol- ogy stem from a need to dominate and control.

Dr. Jonathan Pincus made the observation about serial killers he’s studied: “What I find puzzling is the obsessively repetitious features of the details of the crime in serial killing. Each killer has a specific modus operandi. This be- speaks some kind of perverse need that must be satisfied in a particular man- ner” (2001, p. 129). While Pincus does not specifically refer to the clinical disorder of obsessive compulsiveness, he certainly describes this in the serial killer’s nature as well as his need to kill.

A final consideration is the actual process of completing compulsive acts. First, there is the cycle of discomfort and anxiety, followed by the act (which relieves the tension), followed by a period of guilt and/or a reliving of the act. This process mirrors the apparent cycle of a serial killer’s activities: the urge, the act, and the cooling-off period. The process can also include post-offense behavior, such as reliving the fantasy that has become reality, getting involved with the investigation, or returning to significant locations (Douglas, Ressler, Burgess, & Hartman, 1986).

POSTTRAUMATIC STRESS Another condition to be considered is PTSD. Earlier versions of the DSM

described this condition as experiencing an event outside the range of usual human experience and that would be markedly distressing to almost anyone, which could describe the early experiences of many serial murderers. The DSM-IV-TR goes on to note that the essential feature is the development of characteristic symptoms following exposure to an extreme traumatic stressor involving direct personal experience involving death, serious injury, or a threat to the physical integrity of another person. Other diagnostic features include the experience of intense fear, helplessness or horror, the symptoms of reexperiencing of the traumatic event, persistent avoidance of stimuli related

26 Real-Life Monsters

to the trauma and a numbing of general responsiveness as well as symptoms of increased arousal (2000, p. 463).

Specific behaviors include recurrent and intrusive recollections of the event, including distressing dreams and dissociative states featuring flash- backs. Persistent avoidance of stimuli associated with the trauma include a detachment or estrangement from other people and a markedly reduced abil- ity to feel emotions, especially those associated with intimacy and also show- ing diminished responsiveness to the external world, referred to as “psychic numbing” or “emotional anesthesia,” usually soon after the traumatic event (2000, p. 464). Victims of PTSD also report an inability to have loving feel- ings, a sense of a foreshortened future, irritability, outbursts of anger, and difficulty in concentrating.

The childhood trauma that all serial killers seem to share, whether emo- tional, physical, sexual, or a combination, would most likely fit the description of a distressing event, serious enough to cause these symptoms. Henry Lee Lucas and Andrei Chikatilo are extreme examples. Certainly, the dissociative trances of a Jeffrey Dahmer, Ted Bundy, or an Edmund Kemper could be interpreted as an example of posttraumatic response.

REACTIVE ATTACHMENT DISORDER If there has been a significant development in the study of serial murder

psychopathology since The Psychology of Serial Murder was first published, it’s the research on reactive attachment disorder. While poor attachment has always been considered, research connecting severe attachment issues and possibly psychopathic or sociopathic behaviors has been clear. From the ATTACh (Association for the Treatment and Training of Attachment in Children, 2011) website:

Many children throughout the world do not benefit from adequate parenting during their early years. Their foundation for healthy devel- opment is damaged so they have difficulty in forming loving, lasting, intimate relationships. This condition, known as attachment disorder, can be triggered by abuse, neglect, abandonment, separation from birth parents, birth trauma, maternal depression, chronic illness, frequent moves and placements, and even divorce. Parents find that children with this condition are less responsive to direction, less eager to please and more aggressive. These children are at increased risk for serious psycho- logical problems in adolescence and adulthood.

Attachment pathology is often related to many of the issues discussed in this chapter, such as dissociation, childhood personality disorders, and elements of psychopathology. Children who have been mistreated often develop a disor- ganized attachment style that is related to dissociation and a lack of sense of self (Ingram & Price, 2010, p. 88). This is considered a defense mechanism created in order to protect the self as a response to trauma. Ingram and Price

Clinical Diagnoses and Serial Killer Traits 27

also reference Ogawa’s 1997 observation that insecure attachment, dissocia- tion, a lack of sense of self, and trauma are related to later disassociation in the context of traumatic experiences (p. 89). Connections to later development of borderline personality disorder were also observed.

As well, Ingram and Price observed studies that correlate insecure attach- ment histories to subjects falsely representing their esteem as overly high to overcompensate for an actual feeling of worthlessness. These behaviors often resembled behavior by those exhibiting narcissistic personality disorders. Also noted is the fact that children’s vulnerability to depression increases, particularly when exposed to new interpersonal stressors, if they have developed insecure attachments according to Bowlby’s 1980 attachment theory (2010, p. 217).

Licensed clinical social worker and child and adolescent therapist Cathy Clevenger notes that children suffering from severe attachment pathology are stuck in the security phase, as commonly referenced in Abraham Maslow’s Hierarchy of Needs. “These children have never learned to trust, never devel- oped empathy,” says Clevenger. “They are stuck in survival mode, hoarding food, never experiencing comfort, developing severe control issues because they cannot control their environments or depend on caregivers.” Clevenger states that the severely attachment disordered child will often display person- ality traits similar to the psychopath, most notably manipulativeness, control- oriented behavior, lies and stealing, destructiveness, a lack of impulse control, evidence of the MacDonald triad, hypervigilance, and displays a lack of con- science or remorse (personal communication, 2011; also from the attachment disorder site, 2011).

The most striking confluence of attachment issues and clinical issues re- lated to the serial murderer personality type is the development of a severe lack of empathy, as seen in the APD or most specific, the psychopathic or sociopathic personality found in the serial killer. Ingram and Price observed, “one pathway toward a lack of concern for others might include an early avoidant attachment relationship, instilling a view of others that is threatening or hostile” (2010, p. 92). Ingram noted the correlation between Cleckley’s and Hare’s research on psychopaths noted previously in this chapter. Children with a history of damaged attachment who are prone to develop problematic impulsivity and severe lack of empathy are highly comparable to the violently narcissistic psychopath or sociopath who exemplifies the nature of the serial murderer.

During his 2011 trial, serial murderer Anthony Sowell made statements about his mother’s lack of nurturing when he was a child and talked about his inability to hug his sisters and that he could not hold their hands.

OTHER DISORDERS—ADHD, DEPRESSION, AND BIPOLAR DISORDER

Research has continued to tie attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) to crime. In 2009, a Yale School of Public Health study found that

28 Real-Life Monsters

children with ADHD are more likely to commit crimes as adults (Nadelmann, 2009). After controlling for race, education, and income level, Yale School of Public Health assistant professor Jason Fletcher and UW researcher Barbara Wolfe found that certain types of crime were linked to particular symptoms of ADHD in children ages 5 to 12. A report by the Center for Science in the Public Interest showed a strong relationship between ADHD and risk for criminality, even in the absence of childhood conduct problems as some studies connecting hyperactivity and adult psychopathology state (“ADHD: New evidence of crime link,” 2007). This echoed a 1997 study that found childhood hyperactivity, even when not combined with conduct problems, is a strong risk for later violence and other issues. Studies show that at least 25 percent of prisoners in U.S. prisons suffered from ADHD (Young, 2010).

Much of the connection appears to stem from impulsive behavior, depres- sion, and engaging in risky behavior. However, it should be noted that it appears when individuals with ADHD commit violent crimes, these acts are more likely to be crimes of spontaneous and reactive aggression rather than carefully plotted out offenses. Such crimes are generally impulsive acts driven by a provocation or conflict (Young, 2010). This would be at odds with the profile of the predatory behavior of the prototypical serial killer. However, these killers do demonstrate a need for power and control, a trait shared by young fire setters with “internal imbalances like attention deficit disorder” (Kolko, 2002, p. 224).

Depression can be connected to crime in a number of ways, often in con- junction with other mental disorders and environmental factors. Also, depres- sion has been connected to magnesium deficiency. The activity of serotonin receptors is affected by changes in magnesium levels. This suggests that part of the high rate of violent crime could be mitigated by ending the magnesium deficiencies (Mason, 1998).

A study from Sweden’s Karolinska Institute suggests that bipolar disorder (manic or depressive features) does not increase the risk of committing violent crimes. It states the overrepresentation of individuals with bipolar disorder in violent crime statistics is attributable to concurrent substance abuse (“Bipolar disorder,” 2010). Some previous research has also suggested that patients with bipolar disorder are more likely to behave violently. However, it has been unclear if the violence is due to the bipolar disorder per se or caused by other aspects of the individual’s personality or lifestyle.

INSANITY: AN UNCLEAR CONCEPT Not mentioned as yet is the possibility that serial killers are psychotic, in-

sane, or simply mentally ill. This may be an issue of semantics, as many will say that anyone who commits atrocities such as savage rape, torture, murder, dismemberment, or cannibalism, surely must be crazy. The idea is that, for one to be able to kill and handle dismembered body parts, the person must be insane. This is commonly referred to as the res ipsa loquitur argument—the

Clinical Diagnoses and Serial Killer Traits 29

theory (actually, the thing) speaks for itself (Masters, 1992). It’s a common re- frain among defense attorneys. It was the claim by the defense in the Dahmer insanity claim in 1992 and was no different as made by Anthony Sowell’s at- torney 19 years later—the idea that living in a house with the rotting remains of his murder victims—means the “man is sick in the head” (Sheeran, 2011c).

Some people feel that the insanity defense requirements are purposefully difficult to ensure conviction of violent offenders. This could be a reason- able conclusion in light of changes resulting from the Hinckley case after his attack on Ronald Reagan and his acquittal by reason of insanity in 1983. Tougher requirements resulted including the removal of the volitional prong (conforming conduct) from the prosecution (Packer, 2009) and requiring the defense to provide an affirmative defense in federal cases. Cheney (1992) felt in the Edmund Kemper trial, “everyone [was] afraid to find out that there was something really wrong with the defendant” (p. 190), and she states that the “disparity between medical and legal definitions of insanity perpetuate a fiction which is bizarre and actually harmful, however soothing to some members of the public” (p. 173).

In 2006, Chicago, Illinois serial killer Paul Runge’s insanity defense was rejected after a psychologist testified he was a sexual sadist with little control over his impulses to attack women (Coen, 2006). Earlier at trial, Runge’s de- fense attempted to claim he should be exempt from the death penalty because his obsessive-compulsive sexual sadism could be likened to mental retardation, causing him to not be able to process information or learn from mistakes.

Those on the prosecution’s side will disagree. E. Michael McCann, prosecut- ing the Dahmer case, drove home this point to the jury: “Committing an unnat- ural act, such as having sex with a dead body, does not in itself denote insanity” (O’Donnell, 1992, p. 14). Even the judge at the Dennis Nilsen trial pointed out that “a mind can be evil without being abnormal” (Masters, 1992, p. 269).

What makes the insanity plea so difficult is it is inherently confusing. “It’s an attempt to explain rationally the irrational,” says William Moffit, an Alex- andria, Virginia, defense attorney (Toufexis, 1992, p. 17). The term insanity bears little resemblance to common or even what is considered medical usage. Insanity is, actually, only a legal term, and is certainly not found in a diagnos- tic manual such as the DSM-IV.

Generally, the legal test is whether, at the time the crime was committed, the defendant was suffering from a mental defect that made him incapable of telling right from wrong. Some states also consider whether a defendant’s mental illness impaired his or her ability to control his or her actions. The Dahmer case hinged on this irresistible impulse defense (Toufexis, 1992).

It is often assumed that the defendant must at least be suffering from a psy- chosis, not a personality disorder (i.e., APD, which is usually specifically ex- cluded as a defense), to qualify for an insanity defense. Most states mirror the American Law Institute’s (ALI) insanity standard, which includes the caveat that the terms mental disease and mental defect “do not include an abnor- mality manifested only by repeated criminal or otherwise antisocial conduct.”

30 Real-Life Monsters

This language was specifically intended to prevent defendants identified as sociopaths or psychopaths from using the defense (Packer, 2009, pp. 12–13).

However, a key point to remember is that even the presence of psychosis is not enough. Merely being schizophrenic does not automatically exculpate one from one’s actions. According to the American Law Institute’s standard, it must be proven that the accused’s mental condition was the reason for his or her not being able to “appreciate the wrongfulness” of his or her actions or be “unable to conform his [or her] conduct to the requirements of the law” (Packer, 2009; Smith & Meyer, 1987, p. 389). Case law in the 1950s required that the criminal act must be considered a product of the mental illness; simply the presence of a mental illness was not enough. Another fac- tor is the varied requirements for meeting standards for insanity in different states or in federal court. Requirements of volition, intent, mens rea, prod- ucts of mental disease, diminished capacity, impulses, appreciation, substan- tial capacity, and many other terms vary wildly between different regions and in the minds of different jurors. However, the issue of not guilty by reason of insanity is one for the courts to decide, more specifically, the juries, mainly because the concept of insanity is purely a legal one, not a medical or sci- entific one, decided eventually in each case by citizen jurists, not by expert witnesses.

In Illinois, for instance, the insanity statute or definition is pretty simple: “§ 5-1-11. Insanity. ‘Insanity’ means the lack of a substantial capacity to ap- preciate the criminality of one’s conduct as a result of mental disorder or mental defect” (Illinois General Assembly, 2012).

Very few serial killers are found insane, but some (e.g., DeSalvo, Lucas, Chase, Corona, and Kemper) have been diagnosed as psychotic or schizo- phrenic at some times. All would appear to have some mental disorders. Still, the majority of individuals considered in this book appeared to understand the difference between right and wrong and seemed aware of the circum- stances and results of their actions. Essentially all were aware of the law’s re- quirements enough to conceal their actions.

Rodney Alcala made an interesting observation when asked why he bit a victim’s breasts during an assault: “You’re in an unreasoning situation. Your brain and you just don’t know what to do. It’s not like do this, do that. You’re not reasoning. You’ve lost your ability to reason. You’re not thinking. . . . I raped her” (Lasseter, 2004, p. 19).

Ted Bundy, admitting he was well aware of what he was doing, just made reference to the compulsion:

I don’t have a split personality. I don’t have blackouts. I remember ev- erything I’ve done. [After one killing] we went out for ice cream after eating hamburgers. It wasn’t like I had forgotten or couldn’t remember, but it was just over . . . gone . . . the force wasn’t pushing me anymore.

(Kendall, 1981, p. 175)

Clinical Diagnoses and Serial Killer Traits 31

Dr. Park Dietz (1992b), the widely respected insanity defense expert, noted in a statement following the Dahmer trial:

If the jury had found Mr. Dahmer insane, it would have been open season for sex offenders, because the core of the defense theory was that sexually deviated men cannot control their behavior. . . . [They] are precisely analogous to the disorders found among most child molesters, serial rapists and serial killers, as well as many of those committing sex offenses.

SUMMARY: THE CLINICAL PERSPECTIVE In considering the theories brought out in this section, a pattern emerges.

It seems that while serial killers show many syndromes as described in the field of clinical psychology, “there is no single diagnostic category [at this time] that fits these individuals. The pathology of serial murderer is a separate diag- nostic category” (Apsche, 1993, p. 10). Illinois and Missouri serial killer and rapist Timothy Krajcir simply described himself in a confession as “twisted” (“Sex offender confesses,” 2007).

Labels such as antisocial, borderline, narcissistic, and psychopath do apply; phenomena such as dissociation, doubling, posttraumatic stress, and obsessive- compulsive behavior can be observed. Many of these states have overlapping features and etiology. There are shared biological or neurological aspects in most cases, indicating a possible physical factor. For most serial killers, there definitely appears to be a history of physical, sexual, or mental abuses. Fi- nally, and possibly most important, these killers seem to evidence a pervasive lost sense of self and intimacy, an inadequacy of identity, and a feeling of no control. These could all be factors in a pathology that manifests itself in the ultimate act of control—the murder, and repeated murder, of other human beings.

This page intentionally left blank

Biolog y and Its Effect on Violent Behavior

3

I am a mistake of nature. I deserve to be done away with.

—Andrei Chikatilo

BIOLOGICAL PERSPECTIVES The notion of nature or biology as a key element in social deviance reaches as far back as criminologist Cesare Lombroso in the late 19th century. Lom- broso observed the physical correlations between violent (born) criminals and certain animals, or beasts of prey. His distinction between the born criminal and the occasional criminal (one led to criminality owing to illness or difficult situation) marks the predatory nature of the serial killer.

Soon after Lombroso’s declarations regarding the inherited nature of crim- inal tendencies, confirmatory evidence was provided in a book by sociologist Richard Dugdale. Included was a study of a clan led by two sons who married their illegitimate sisters; the results showed that out of more than 700 descen- dants, only 6 did not become prostitutes or criminals. Another sociologist, Henry H. Goddard, studied a soldier who had fathered a baby by a feeble- minded girl, then married a Quaker girl from an honest and intelligent fam- ily. Nearly five hundred of the Quaker girl’s descendants were traced, none of whom were criminals; of the same number of descendants of the feeble- minded girl, only 10 percent were normal (Wilson, 1989, pp. 177–179).

Research has grown throughout the years and continued to indicate that aggressiveness and criminality do have a genetic factor (Pervin, 1989).

34 Real-Life Monsters

Identical twins are twice as likely as fraternal twins to be similar in their crimi- nal activity. A close relationship has also been found between antisocial be- havior in adopted children and such behavior in their biological parents. A pioneer in this field, Dr. Adrian Raine of the University of Pennsylvania, states on his Department of Criminology Faculty profile (2011) that his main area of interest is “ Neurocriminology —an emerging sub-discipline of criminology which applies neuroscience to probe the causes and cures of crime.” Dr. Raine and his colleagues’ research focus, which includes “childhood conduct disor- der, reactive and proactive aggression, adult antisocial personality disorder, homicide, and psychopathy,” simply continues to pave a path in this explod- ing arena of research.

One fascinating observation made by Raine and Sanmartin (2001) was a measure of what might be more influential, deprived family environment or poor brain functioning. His research included the positron emission tomog- raphy (PET) scans of murderers of a control group, a group of deprived (poor home life) murderers and those from a relatively good household. The PET scans of the normal or control group and the murderers in the deprived group looked most alike, whereas the scans of the murderers from the good homes were markedly different. The inference was that the brain functioning of the murderers influenced by a bad environment was reasonably normal, but the killers from a good environment were more influenced by the abnormal, lack of prefrontal brain functioning than they were by their environments (pp. 43–44). Raine found that the murderers from good homes showed a 14.2 percent rate of reduced functioning of the right orbitofrontal cortex, a brain area they found was of particular interest. According to Raine, “damage to this brain area in previously well-controlled adults results in personality and emotional deficits that parallel criminal psychopathic behavior” or what other researchers termed “acquired sociopathy” (p. 44).

An analysis of case histories shows a steady pattern of inherited biolog- ical and/or physical abnormalities in serial killers. Many such killers, over the course of their abusive upbringings, suffered head injuries and trauma directly—for example, Henry Lee Lucas, Albert DeSalvo, and Bobby Joe Long. Head injuries have been known to cause markedly abnormal personal- ity changes, as well as can affect higher brain functions, such as mediation of instincts (as in rage, aggression, violence, and sexual gratification). The cerebral disturbances of some individuals are detected by neurological signs as temporal lobe epilepsy and electroencephalogram (EEG) abnormalities. John Wayne Gacy is just one serial killer diagnosed with epilepsy. Others exhibit irregular EEGs under special circumstances—for example, after drinking al- cohol (Levin & Fox, 1985, p. 31). One killer’s abnormal EEG was referred to as a “neurophysiological handicap” that weakened his ability to resist the psy- chogenetically induced impulse to kill (Revitch & Schlesinger, 1981, p. 22).

There is a striking prevalence of neurological impairment among juvenile killers. In a study done by Dr. Dorothy Otnow-Lewis, all 14 of the death row inmates in her sample had a history of symptoms consistent with brain damage, including head injuries severe enough to result in hospitalization

Biolog y and Its Effect on Violent Behavior 35

and/or indention of the cranium. In addition, serious documented neuro- logical abnormalities such as focal brain injury, abnormal head circumfer- ence, abnormal reflexes, seizure disorders, and abnormal EEG readings were found (Ewing, 1990, p. 9).

Other neurological signals include epilepsy (as in the case of Gacy), dyslexia, and other learning disorders. A classic example is Bobby Joe Long. His con- genital dysfunction of the endocrine system caused him to develop breasts at puberty, and according to Norris (1992), experience a lunar protomenstrual cycle for life. It is noted some experts disagree that such a cycle could even exist. Combined with his brain injuries from a motorcycle accident and four other severe head traumas before the age of 10, this condition must have had an impact on his insatiable sex drive, persistent headaches, and violent personality.

THE MACDONALD TRIAD Another support for the theory of physical abnormality is the presence

of behavior clusters commonly referred to as the MacDonald triad . These behaviors include late enuresis (bed-wetting, later than five years of age), fire setting, and animal abuse and torture. MacDonald studied 48 psychotic and 52 non-psychotic patients and found that “very sadistic patients often had [these] three in common in their childhood histories” (Merz-Perez & Heide, 2004, p. 6).

Various researchers since MacDonald’s 1963 study have asserted that these characteristics, as well as other displays of impulse control possibly traced to a neurological origin, can be predictive of future violence (Revitch & Schlesinger, 1981, p. 177). As cited in Slavkin (2001), “the co-morbidity of these behaviors and their predictive power in identifying adult criminal behavior have been verified in a number of studies” (Lester, 1975; Prentky & Carter, 1984; Robbins & Robbins, 1967; Rothstein, 1963; Wax & Haddox, 1974) . As far back as 1940, Yarnell (cited in Slavkin, 2001) called this group- ing an ego triad, observing problems with enuresis and cruelty to animals in young fire setters. These specific control issues are further discussed within the context of the triad in Chapter 4.

While MacDonald’s study resulted in his observation that very sadistic pa- tients shared the common characteristics of enuresis, fire setting, and tor- turing small animals, he was not convinced that the appearance of the triad was a valuable predictor of future homicidal behavior (Merz-Perez & Heide, 2004). However, Hellman and Blackman’s (1966) study concluded that the triad was important in predicting violent antisocial behavior when noted in childhood. Their argument stated that the voiding represented in enuresis equated in fantasy with damaging and destroying, representing sadism and hostility. They said fire setting was a “manifestation of the type of aggres- sion associated with enuresis” and that the two characteristics were intimately related. Hellman and Blackman also concluded the torture of dogs and cats, which “violated the human bond with pets,” was a more significant predictor of future violent behavior than the torture of other small animals such as turtles and flies (pp. 6–7).

36 Real-Life Monsters

TRAUMA TO THE BRAIN Brain injuries litter the landscape of violent behavior. Earle Leonard Nel-

son, known as the “Gorilla man,” murdered almost two dozen women in the mid-1920s. He was thrown from a trolley when he was 10 and lay comatose for nearly a week. His behavior reportedly became even more bizarre from that point on (Schechter & Everitt, 1997, p. 111). He complained of head- aches, memory loss, and was increasingly aggressive.

Dr. Adrian Raine’s 1994 brain-scan study, discussed earlier, revealed that adults convicted of violent crimes showed impaired function in a key area of the brain linked to impulse control (Baron-Cohen, 2011; Elias, 1994). The findings add to the growing evidence that biological qualities may predispose a person to violent acts. However, Raine noted, “that doesn’t mean these brain functions aren’t caused by the environment.” Dr. Raine said the impair- ment could be inborn and/or caused by a variety of experiences, including violent shaking by adults in childhood, concussions, gunshot wounds, or even bad falls.

Dr. Raine did PET brain scans on 22 adults arrested for murder or at- tempted murder. Each exam was compared to the scan of a matched adult of the same age, but who had never been accused of a violent crime. Findings showed evidence of significantly fewer active cells—meaning lower function in two brain areas crucial to impulse control that are located in the prefrontal cortex. No other brain dysfunctions were found.

Dr. Dorothy Otnow-Lewis, in describing some of the “overkill” of vio- lent offenses found in the case of “Lucky” Larson, compared his actions to a “decorticate cat” (Otnow-Lewis, 1998, pp. 125–126). She explained a cat with a cortex surgically separated from the rest of the brain appears normal at first glance. It purrs and responds to affection. The doctor states the cat’s “responses to stimuli that ordinarily would cause expressions of mild discomfort or annoyance are no longer moderated by the frontal cortex.” This stimulation causes the cat to become “ferocious, directing its attack at anything it perceives as threatening or uncomfortable.” The 54 stab wounds Lucky Larson inflicted on a hapless convenience store clerk, according to Otnow-Lewis and her colleague, Dr. Jonathan Pincus, were the result of “the expression of a limbic system released from higher corti- cal control” and should not be “held completely responsible for behaviors beyond his control.”

Otnow-Lewis’s study included testimony about Johnny Garrett, who was executed in Texas in 1992 for the rape and murder of a nun when he was 17. Johnny’s cousin stated:

Grandma hit me in the head with a pipe. . . . Grandma hit Johnny in the head a lot of times. . . . She would just up and hit him with whatever she had close by. . . . Grabbed one of those things that you put in shoes and hit him in the head with that thing until he went to sleep. That’s what

Biolog y and Its Effect on Violent Behavior 37

she did when she wanted us to go to sleep. She would hit us in the head until we passed out.

(Otnow-Lewis, 1998, p. 263)

Otnow-Lewis drew a comparison between the brain damage of Lucky Lar- son with another murderer, Marie Moore. She noted that the medical history of Moore underlined a brain disorder. She stated that a computerized axial tomography (CAT ) scan revealed “a very striking pattern of frontal lobe at- rophy with widening of the interhemispheric fissure and some lesser atrophy of the vermis of the cerebellum,” according to the attending neuroradiologist (Otnow-Lewis, 1998, pp. 163–164). Otnow-Lewis wondered if the damage was a result of a car accident Moore was involved in when she was hit by the windshield, or possibly from a previous attack with a baseball bat. Her his- tory also included complaints of blackouts, memory lapses, enuresis, violent episodes, unexplained thefts as a child, buzzing in her ears, passing out, and body numbness (p. 164). At one point, her childhood neurologist declared her problems were emotional, but Otnow-Lewis points to her frontal lobe damage as yet another example of erratic behavior, most notably her shack- ing-up with a very young man and inferred that their kidnapping, torture, and murder of a young girl was related to brain damage.

The effects of cumulative brain trauma on behavior continue to find their way into the news. Athletes such as college football player Owen Thomas and former National Football League (NFL) Chicago Bears’ star Dave Duerson experienced depression, erratic behavior, and committed suicide in 2010 and 2011, respectively. Postmortem brain analyses revealed a degenerative condi- tion described as chronic traumatic encephalopathy (CTE). This condition has been found in more than 20 deceased NFL players (Silcox, 2010) and high school and college football players as well as wrestlers, boxers, and more recently National Hockey League (NHL) players such as Bob Probert and Reggie Fleming, both known for their aggressive, physical play (“Research- ers,” 2011). In a tragically short period of time, three former enforcers in the NHL died in the summer of 2011: Wade Belak, Derek Boogaard, and Rick Rypien, the latter two in their 20s and Belak only 35. Rypien had suffered from depression for years and Belak, who hanged himself, appeared to as well. Boogaard suffered multiple concussions in his years as a hockey fighter and his family donated his brain to the same Boston University where researchers discovered the evidence of CTE in Fleming and Probert. Former NHL tough guy Stu Grimson commented that if science shows a prolonged career in the role of enforcer has a high correlation to traumatic brain disease, “That’s not going to come as a surprise to anybody” (Brady & Allen, 2011, 10C).

Professional wrestler Chris Benoit’s story was the most tragic. In 2007 over a three-day period, Benoit strangled his wife and son, and hung himself. While different theories flew regarding his behavior, it was determined that drugs, most notably steroids, were not a causal factor. However, a fellow

38 Real-Life Monsters

wrestler contacted Chris Benoit’s father and suggested that the cumulative years of brain injury could have led to his crimes. After analysis, West Virginia University head of neurosurgery Julian Bailes stated Benoit’s brain was so damaged that “it resembled the brain of an 85-year-old Alzheimer’s patient,” and said it was something you should never see in a 40-year-old (“Benoit’s brain,” 2007). The damage was compared to the similar brain results found in the NFL players’ studies.

GENETICS AND BEHAVIOR It has been said that the predatory behavior of prey animals reflects “a neu-

rological basis that is different from that of other kinds” of behavior (Moyer, 1968). In other words, predatory aggression is different from other aggres- sion, in that it “does not show rage and is not interchangeable with fight behavior, but it is purpose-oriented, accurately aimed, and the tension ends with the accomplishment of the goal” (Fromm, 1973, p. 99).

The calm, purposeful behavior of the accomplished serial murderer clearly reflects the actions of a predatory aggressor rather than the behavior of an ex- cited, fight-stimulated organism. This behavior is best described by researchers like Baron-Cohen (2011) and Blair et al. (2005) who describe the nonempa- thetic, goal-oriented criminal aggression as “instrumental aggression.”

The temptation is great to consider a person’s history of violence as the main precursor to further violence. However, not every child who is abused becomes a serial killer, just as not every child who is abused develops a multi- ple personality. Sometimes children in the same family, subjected to the same abuse, take different psychological routes. One may develop a multiple per- sonality and another, although experiencing problems, may not. These mixed results are also true of children raised in parental surrounding that would either suggest the development of a psychopath or not. There is an explana- tion for these differences, and it appears to be organic. Baron-Cohen (2011) in The Science of Evil: On Empathy and the Origins of Cruelty feels this is an indication of the environment interacting with “genes for empathy” in pro- ducing the psychopath, genes that evidence exists are associated with scoring on various measures of empathy (pp. 126–127).

So, what about the children of killers, or of rapists? There are few twin studies to ascertain the behavior of siblings raised in different settings involv- ing violence. However, consider the example of Aileen Wuornos, an alcoholic lesbian slayer of seven. Her father was a child molester, a kidnapper, and a “violent sexual predator” (Reynolds, 1992, p. 257). He was also a bed-wetter until age 13. At one point, he escaped from a hospital for the criminally in- sane, but he eventually hung himself in prison. When she was an infant, he left Aileen and her 15-year-old mother. Little Aileen must have been her father’s daughter. Her dad may not have taught her anything, but did he leave the seed of violence?

Biolog y and Its Effect on Violent Behavior 39

It should be noted that nearly all the studies of empathy in twins have found a greater correlation on empathy measures in monozygotic or identi- cal twins when compared to dizygotic or nonidentical twins (Baron-Cohen, 2011, p. 128).

Extensive research on biological factors regarding serial killers was con- ducted by Richard T. Kraus (1995), in an investigation of the Arthur Shaw- cross case. Kraus noted that the 47, XYY chromosomal karyotype, abnormally elevated urinary kryptopyrroles, and multiple brain injuries “have relevance as identifiable precursors for potential violence in such individuals with a history of behavioral disturbance.”

Jacobs, Brunton, and Melville (1965) initiated the first chromosome sur- vey for XYY males, discovering a high incidence of males with the extra chromosome among a criminal population described as dangerous and vi- olent. Later studies (Casey, Segall, Street, & Blank, 1966; Court-Brown, Price, & Jacobs, 1968; Price & Whatmore, 1967) supported these findings, and concluded that “the extra Y chromosome is associated with anti-social behavior . . . and predispose its carriers to increased risk for developing a psychopathic personality” (Kraus, 1995, pp. 11–24). Also of interest were findings by Neilsen et al. indicating that XYY patients might be a “com- paratively high risk for committing arson, sexual criminality and a high fre- quency of violence” (Neilsen, 1970; Neilsen, Tsuboi, Turver, Jensen, & Sachs, 1969). Price and Jacobs (1970) found that “the behavior disorders in these men which may exist in the absence of mental deficiency . . . correlate with a personality disorder . . . [and] points to the existence of a constitu- tional psychopathic state” (p. 365).

In case reports of children with XYY, the children are described as “enig- matic in their personality development . . . vulnerable to simple threats and stresses that most would shrug off . . . loners . . . isolationists” (Money, 1970). Zeuthen, Hansen, Christensen, and Neilsen (1975) found the children with XYY who “grew up in good homes . . . to a certain extent differed from their siblings”; they were “more impulsive, restless, hot tempered, hyperactive . . . and lacked control of aggressive impulses.”

Behavioral Genetics (1982) summed it up: “For the XYY, there seems to be little doubt. The extra Y does create some special risk for developing anti- social behavior.” These findings all suggest episodes and characteristics in the histories of most serial killers. It is also true that the XYY violence theory has its supporters and detractors alike.

Kryptopyrrole (referred to as the mauve factor) is an endogenous metabo- lite that occurs in humans in either very low amounts or not at all. A read- ing of Arthur Shawcross’s kryptopyrrole level revealed the following: “urine kryptopyrrole: H 200.66 mcg/lOOcc. Expected value 0–20” (Olsen, 1993, p. 491). The H was laboratory shorthand for high, already evident by the numbers. Shawcross had more than 10 times the expected highest amount of kryptopyrrole circulating in his body.

40 Real-Life Monsters

When this substance circulates in the body, it forms a stable Shiff base with pyridoxal phosphate (the aldehyde form of pyridoxine or vitamin B6, and then complexes with zinc, thereby depriving the body of these two essential com- pounds (Pfeiffer, Sohler, Jenny, & Iliev, 1974). Both pyridoxal phosphate and zinc are cofactors at the catalytic site of many enzymes. Decarboxylation reac- tions normally involve pyridoxal phosphate in the synthesis of various neu- rotransmitters, such as dopamine, norepinephirine, gamma-aminobutyric acid, and serotonin, while zinc is a cofactor in many enzymes, such as lactate dehy- drogenase and alkaline phosphate. In addition, both pyridoxal phosphate and zinc are involved in the biosynthesis of heme, which is essential to life ( Harper’s Biochemistry, 1990). As a result, any deficiencies in pyridoxal phosphate or zinc can result in medical illness and psychiatric disturbance (Kraus, 1995).

In a study of the relationships among kryptopyrrole, zinc, and vitamin B6, Ward (1975) reported that the level of kryptopyrrole can vary in the same individual, increasing when that person is experiencing more stress and falling “dramatically” with large doses of zinc and vitamin B6 with an associated de- crease in stress. Pfeiffer (1974) states that urinary excretion of kryptopyrrole is increased by stress of any kind.

O’Reilly, Hughes, Russell, and Ernest (1965) found that the incidence of this condition was “much higher in emotionally disturbed children and adults than in the general population.” A high urinary kryptopyrrole level does ap- pear to correlate with low stress tolerance and loss of control (Kraus, 1995). Thus, it is considered a “biochemical marker of psychiatric dysfunction” and “can identify individuals at high risk for becoming violent.”

Kraus’s extensive, ground-breaking research in the Shawcross case clearly indicates that there are biological markers for psychiatric disturbance and vio- lence. Also indicated is the aggravation caused by stress at all levels. The XYY research is extensive, whereas kryptopyrrole study continues to be limited regarding serial killers at this time. However, the inference of a biological predisposition is inescapable.

Baron-Cohen’s (2011) research on the “empathy genes” resulted in his conclusion that four genes, after genotyping took place, showed “strongly significant association” with the empathy quotient, a measure of empathy in an individual. These genes were: CYPB11B1, a gene from the sex steroid group; WFSI, located in the group related to social–emotional behavior; and two from the “neural growth group, NTRK1 and GARBR3” (p. 138).

Researchers and geneticists at both Massachusetts General Hospital and the Netherlands found a genetic mutation in some men that was more likely to cause them to be aggressive and violent (“Dutch,” 1993). Their reports stated that the mutation is associated with abnormal behavior, including at- tempted rape and exhibitionism (Snider, 1993). They found by urinalysis, the men abnormally metabolized the enzyme monoamine oxidase A (MAOA). In the brain, MAOA breaks down dopamine, serotonin, and noradrenaline, all substances known to affect behavior. When the researchers examined family genes, the men had slightly different coding from unaffected males.

Biolog y and Its Effect on Violent Behavior 41

Another thought is, the previously considered personality disorders—APD, borderline personality disorder, and obsessive compulsiveness—all suggest some genetic or biological link in serial killers and further underscore the pos- sibility of a physical defect or disposition. As well, childhood disorders with the closest links to crime (ADHD, oppositional defiant disorder, and conduct disorder) suggest genetic inheritance in recent studies (Rowe, 2002, p. 39).

Unfortunately, as things stand there are still no reliable, predictive tests for the criminal brain at this time. Rowe (2002) and others have suggested the possibility of genes that could be related to criminal disposition, such as the dopamine (D4) receptor, serotonin, and MAOA. Raine’s 2000 magnetic resonance imaging tests included testing the resting heart rates and skin con- ductance levels of subjects in 2000 and found they predicted whether some- one had antisocial personality disorder with 77 percent accuracy, a 27 percent improvement over guessing (Rowe, 2002, p. 85). It has been established by multiple researchers that an impaired functioning of the prefrontal cortex of the brain is related to criminal disposition. Still,

Although modern brain imagining technologies can produce wonder- fully detailed images of the living, functioning brain, they cannot pick out the criminally disposed from the nondisposed with anything like a diagnostic level of accuracy.

(Rowe, 2002, p. 71)

According to Stout, “over and over again, heritability studies come up with a statistical finding that has emotionally charged social and political implications—that indeed a person’s tendency to possess certain sociopathic characteristics is partially born in the blood, perhaps as much as 50 percent so” (2005, pp. 123–124). However, general acceptance of biological behav- iors for criminal behavior continues to be slow, regardless of increasing stud- ies suggesting otherwise. As criminologists Williams and McShane (1999) state regarding public policy, “biological versions have not been very popular in the past half-century,” and “until recently there have been few biologi- cally oriented crime policies” (p. 46). Meanwhile, “psychological perspec- tives have found much more favor with policymakers as a standard approach to criminal behavior.”

This page intentionally left blank

Environment, Background, and Personality

4

They ain’t got, I don’t think, a human being alive that can say he had the childhood I had.

—Henry Lee Lucas

POSSIBLE ETIOLOGICAL FACTORS In considering the potential ingredients to produce a serial murderer, the lit- erature includes many phenomena in addition to the syndromes described in the DSM-IV . Mental, physical, and sexual abuse; organic damage or biologi- cal anomaly; mental and attitude maladjustments; and sexual dysfunction are but some of the other factors that come into play.

This chapter reviews the issues that seem to consistently color or correlate with the histories of serial killers. Some issues are included because of an in- tuitive sense one develops when analyzing the literature; others simply appear too often in these cases to ignore. Here, the focus is on the environmental details and the social influences on the development of the serial murderer. Unlike Chapter 2, which presented clinical psychology’s perspective on the personality types embodied by these offenders, this chapter discusses the ex- ternal and social issues in their development.

ENVIRONMENTAL FACTORS Clearly deserving of equal consideration in the development of serial killers

is the matter of environmental setting or history. The trauma experienced by

44 Real-Life Monsters

the majority of the killers in question is legendary. Consider Albert DeSalvo, who watched his father savagely beat his mother, witnessed the murders of drunks in his neighborhood, and was eventually sold along with his sister to a farmer as slaves. Then there is Henry Lee Lucas, who was forced to watch his mother have sex with various men, was beaten mercilessly daily, was made to eat from the floor and steal food, and was brought up as a girl until age seven, wearing long hair and dressing in girl’s clothes (Egger, 2002).

Gerald Stano, who confessed to 25 murders of young women in Florida, was linked to at least 40 more. He was the fifth child born to a mother who lost all her children to adoption because of abuse and neglect. When Stano was removed from his home, he was malnourished, physically and emotion- ally neglected, and functioning at an “animalistic” level (Sears, 1991, p. 37).

There are many less dramatic instances of negative environmental settings, but certainly they are abhorrent enough to cause serious damage to a person’s sense of self or to development of an appreciation of the lives of others. The beatings that the father of John Wayne Gacy gave him for his suspected ho- mosexuality and underachievement; the practice of Bobby Joe Long’s mother making him live in a hotel room with her, sharing her bed; the ridicule and punishment Edmund Kemper received from his mother and grandmother, questioning his masculinity—are just a few examples. The combination of physical predisposition and environmental stressors helps develop a pattern of maladjustment with two major consequences: a distorted sense of self and a dysfunctional sexual component.

ESTEEM DEVELOPMENT AND SENSE OF SELF Along with physical abuse, the childhoods of most serial killers are filled

with systematic emotional rape that harms their impressionable psyches. This injury prohibits them from developing a healthy sense of self, an understand- ing of intimacy, or feelings of personal esteem.

Childhood is when these killers develop obsessive and distorted views of their own identities and their ever-increasing need for control. Most have had little, if any, control over themselves or their surroundings as children, and their resulting fear and dread in relation to control issues is understandable, if not predictable. Ingram and Price (2010) make note of research suggest- ing child mistreatment and other traumas in general have been found to be related to dissociation, and a lack of sense of self (p. 88). Also noted are sev- eral ways in which dissociation, a lack of sense of self, trauma, and insecure attachment may be related (p. 89). On the other end of the continuum is an inflated or artificial self-view, characterizing additional personality disorders such as narcissism.

Many of the personality disorders discussed in Chapter 2 involve a dis- torted sense of self and a lack of control. These disorders can overlap with other issues, such as fear of loneliness, rejection, overreaction to stress, and misuse of alcohol and other substances.

Environment, Background, and Personality 45

Many killers, such as Kenneth Bianchi, Rodney Alcala, Aileen Wuornos, David Berkowitz, and Ted Bundy, were adopted early in life. Adoption is sometimes viewed by the child as the ultimate form of rejection by his parents. Certainly, Jeffrey Dahmer’s tumultuous upbringing and perceived abandon- ment set into motion his loss of self and his immobilizing fear of being alone.

In a classic 1985 FBI study of sexually oriented murderers (Ressler, Bur- gess, Depue, Douglas, & Hazelwood, 1985), family histories were found to consistently lack a process for the subjects, as children, to become adults and relate to and value other members of society. Inadequate patterns of relating and infrequent positive interaction with family members were noted. A high degree of instability in the home life, as well as a poor-quality attachment among family members, was also found. Also, interviews showed that most offenders had unsatisfactory relationships with their fathers, while reporting that relationships with their mothers were of highly ambivalent quality. Today, researchers would certainly develop the attachment issues suggested there.

Revitch and Schlesinger (1981) found that, in cases of sexually motivated compulsive gynocide (the murder of women), there was some unhealthy emotional involvement with the mother. They felt that this resulted in a dis- placement of affect from mother to other women, culminating in a displaced matricide (p. 174). They also quote Freud, who stated that “the sexual instinct itself may not be something simple, that it may be on the contrary, be com- posed of many components, some of which form perversions. Our clinical ob- servation thus calls our attention to fusions, which have lost their expression in the uniform normal behavior” (1938, p. 175). Revitch and Schlesinger note that in sadistic gynocide there is a fusion of sex and aggression.

THE SEXUAL COMPONENT A consistent factor in the development of the serial killer is the presence

of a seriously dysfunctional sexual orientation. This is another issue given varying degrees of importance by different researchers. Some feel it is a key influence, whereas others view it as merely incidental. The position of this author is that deviant sexual motivation clearly has an impact on the killer’s psychology, that it is the bridge, the clearest link between mental and physical processes in the psychopathology in question. Dr. David Abrahamson main- tains that in “all of what we call senseless or aimless violence, there is a strong sexual element” (Cheney, 1992, p. 210). Defense expert Dr. George Woods, testifying at Anthony Sowell’s trial, stated categorically many serial murder cases “have a strong sexual component to them” (Scott, 2011b).

Westley Allan Dodd, the child killer and rapist put to death in Washington State, was certainly on his way to a successful career as a serial sex killer. In his 1992 self-written pamphlet to parents, teachers, and children to warn against people like him ( When You Meet a Stranger ), he claimed to have molested about 25 boys and girls and attempted 40 others. Albert DeSalvo’s uncontrollable sex drive seemed to fuel his mania, as he committed an estimated two thousand

46 Real-Life Monsters

sexual assaults (Leyton, 1986). Most killers’ careers are launched by violent sexual fantasies that lead to the sexual assault preceding the actual murders.

Some murderers do not kill with the overt motivation of sexual activity such as rape or sexual assault (pre- or postmortem) seeming to be a factor; however, other fetishes and sexual paraphilia are usually apparent. Ted Bundy had an abnormal collection of socks and once stated the importance of having more socks than he could ever use. This reflected the foot fetishes of Dayton Rogers and Jerry Brudos, both of whom severed the feet of some of their victims for later enjoyment.

Not to be forgotten is the motive of pure sexual sadism. Sadism, torture, and rape are usually thought of in terms of violence, but the sexual aspects of these crimes should not be overlooked. Freud referred to sadism as a partial drive of the libido, and explained that sadistic desires have no overt connec- tion with sexual strivings, but are unconsciously motivated by them (Fromm, 1973, p. 280). Quite often the crimes of the killer enmesh the violence and sexual excitement of the sadist.

Edmund Kemper was described as a pure sadist by one psychiatrist, a con- dition presumably fueled by his hatred of his mother. DeSalvo seemed to strike out at women who represented the cold rebuke of his wife, Irmagard. Angelo Buono was a seasoned pro at brutal rape before his first murder for pleasure. Even Richard Ramirez noted in an interview ( Inside Edition, 1993) that his sexual satisfaction could be reached only through violence.

However, sadistic specialists such as Lawrence Bittaker and Roy Norris just tortured and mutilated for the sheer enjoyment of hearing their victims scream. Bittaker and Norris wanted to stock their own private town of young girls to torture and rape. Herman Mudgett killed just to “hear their cries for mercy” (Editors of Time-Life Books, 1992, p. 95). Dayton Rogers even stopped attacking one victim who ceased screaming and resisting, apparently because she took the whole pleasure out of the act.

Albert Fish was so enthralled with sexual sadism and masochism that he regularly inserted needles in his groin, along with engaging in various other forms of self-mutilation. He looked forward to the electric chair as the “su- preme thrill, the only one I haven’t tried” (Editors of Time-Life Books, 1992, p. 99). Sexual sadists have displayed interest in activities consistent with the serial murderer, such as:

selection of strangers as victims; advance selection of a location to which the victim is taken; participation of a partner; careful planning (includ- ing impersonation of a police officer); use of a pretext in approaching victims; keeping victims captive . . . sexual bondage . . . performing multiple sex acts . . . intentional torture; murder or serial killings (most often by strangulation); concealing victims’ corpses; recording offenses; and keeping personal items belonging to the victims.

(Dietz, Hazelwood, & Warren, 1990)

Environment, Background, and Personality 47

There is also the violent homosexual, who combines the components of a decayed, personal loathing sense of self and sexual urges. Homosexuality is barely referenced in the DSM-IV-TR, only when the individual experi- ences “persistent or marked distress about sexual orientation” (APA, 2000, p. 582). Killers who are fueled by a rage and a hatred of their own sexuality include sexually inadequate or homosexual individuals such as serial kill- ers Dean Corll, Westley Dodd, John Wayne Gacy, Andrei Chikatilo, Larry Eyler, possibly Jeffrey Dahmer, Dennis Nilsen, and spree killer Andrew Cu- nanan. They are prime examples of such violent expression of frustration and rage.

Patrick Kearney, referring to younger homosexuals as “hustlers and pho- nies,” admitted to killing at least 32 young men and boys, ranging from ages 5 to 28. He butchered them for years, later at a rate of one per month, and earned the nickname the “Trash Bag Killer” for his disposal methods of dumping the severed remains in garbage bags (Cartel, 1985, pp. 145, 147). Forensic psychiatrist Richard Rappaport echoed observations of personal rage between killer and victim when he noted that “a serial killer often has an ex- tremely close relationship with his victims. He sees in them characteristics he sees in himself” (Garelik & Maranto, 1984).

Lucas, Toole, Bianchi, Buono, Long, Bittaker, Morris, Dahmer, Bundy, Shawcross, Gacy, Corona, Dodd, Chikatilo, Rader, Alcala, Sowell—the list goes on endlessly of serial killers whose primary interest included some form of sexual activity. More recently, a Canadian Air Force Officer, Col. Russell Williams, entrusted with flying prime ministers and Queen Elizabeth II, was exposed as living a double life: secretly acting as a serial killer as well as prac- ticing a shocking fetish for stealing and photographing himself wearing girls’ and women’s underwear while masturbating (Noronha, 2010).

A popular theory is that rape and sadism are not acts of sex but of violence. Possibly, but not in all cases. It’s probably somewhat dangerous to dismiss the personal sexual preference nature of these offenders. In some extreme cases, acts of torture, rape, necrophilia, and other deviant paraphilic activities may be labeled as acts of violence, but must also be recognized as acts, however distorted, of sex.

Causative aspects of a sexual deviance, usually including some form of paraphilia or sexual activity with a nonconsenting partner (i.e., rape, exhibi- tionism, or pedophilia), can reflect common psychological characteristics. So- cial isolation, low self-esteem, and feelings of sexual inadequacy can indicate an emotional immaturity (Costello & Costello, 1992, p. 272).

Sadistic tendencies can sometimes be traced to an early association of emo- tional feelings in response to someone’s inflicting pain, or even torturing an animal. When the experience is a vivid, haunting one, the result may link the inflicting of pain with sexual arousal. Masochism seems to be triggered by early experiences of extreme pain linked with strong emotion, which in some way is associated with a satisfying sexual event (Costello & Costello, 1992, p. 269). Consistent with a diathesis–stress model (see Chapter 5), a

48 Real-Life Monsters

predisposing biological component or personality trait must be assumed to cause such events, however powerful, to have lasting effects.

Certainly, the dimension that separates these criminals from ordinary rap- ists and sexual offenders is the eventual killing of their victims. The act ap- pears far more significant than just a way to cover up a crime or silence a witness. Instead, it was for the expressed pleasure and catharsis of the act itself, whether to dominate, to control, or, as Ted Bundy put it, “to possess them . . . forever.”

FANTASY A deviant and consuming fantasy appears to be the fuel that fires the pro-

cess. Serial killers seem, at an early age, to become immersed in a deep state of fantasy, often losing track of the boundaries between fantasy and reality. They dream of dominance, control, sexual conquest, violence, and eventually murder. Fantasy would seem to be the place where the killer retreats in a dis- sociative episode.

Long Island serial killer Joel Rifkin was consumed by violent fantasies. After a childhood of rejection and isolation, he started having daydreams about raping and stabbing women. Inspired by the Hitchcock film Frenzy, he became fixated on the idea of strangling prostitutes (“Joel Rifkin biography,” 2011). He eventually acted out in this obsession.

Jeffrey Dahmer, in psychiatric interviews, revealed that he fantasized how it would feel to attack a jogger and “sexually enjoy him” (Dietz, 1992a). He thought out his plan of stalking someone in a certain place and subse- quently went there with a baseball bat in rehearsal of his plan. After he was imprisoned, Dahmer spoke with Robert Ressler (Ressler & Shachtman, 1997, p. 137), who asked him about fantasy and what was about to happen before he went out to hunt for a victim:

D AHMER : Just . . . using pictures of past victims. The videotapes, the pornography videotapes, the magazines.

R ESSLER : But there was a continuation [of the fantasy by] using these things, the skulls, things of that nature?

D AHMER : Right.

At an early age, Edmund Kemper also had fantasies of killing his sisters and other people. He extensively fantasized and carefully rehearsed, going through the motions of picking up hitchhikers before he would eventually carry out his homicidal impulses. Dahmer’s and Kemper’s activities are exam- ples of Prentky et al.’s (1989) idea of “rehearsal fantasy,” a motivator that is practiced in the environment. This rehearsal fantasy is usually associated with dysfunctions such as genital and gender dysphoria, a general feeling of unhap- piness and anxiety regarding sexual issues, experienced by both Dahmer and Kemper. It is in the fantasy where the serial killer determines his future vic- tims’ goodness of fit, a mental representation of the type of individual needed to fulfill a particular pathological need.

Environment, Background, and Personality 49

Many studies have supported the role that fantasy plays in motivating the serial murderer. The Prentky et al. (1989) study, in which samples of serial murderers versus single sexual murderers were compared, postulated the role of fantasy as an internal drive mechanism for repetitive (serial) acts of sexual violence. The study’s hypothesis was that three items common to serial of- fenders would be manifested by the existence of a drive mechanism, described as an intrusive fantasy life. They included a high prevalence of paraphilias, documented or self-reported violent fantasies, and organized crime scenes. All three hypotheses were supported by the study.

MacCulloch, Snowden, and Wood (1983, p. 29) found “a pattern of sadistic fantasies that, in repetition-compulsion fashion, were played out repeatedly—initially in fantasy only, later on in behavioral mock trials, and eventually in assaults. The more the fantasies were cognitively rehearsed, the more power they acquired.” They found that “once the restraints inhibiting the acting out of the fantasy are no longer present, the individual is likely to engage in a series of progressively more accurate ‘trial runs’ in an attempt to enact the fantasy as imagined.”

The genesis and pervasiveness of such fantasies may result from a failure to master the impulses stimulated by child abuse and trauma, hence the repeti- tive and compulsive nature of both the fantasy and the serial sexual violence (Grossman, 1991).

Burgess, Hartman, and Ressler (1986) found a fantasy-based motivational model for sexual homicide. Interactive components included impaired de- velopment or attachments in early life; formative traumatic events; patterned responses that serve to generate fantasies; a private, internal world consumed with violent thoughts that leaves the person isolated and preoccupied; and a feedback filter sustaining repetitive thinking patterns. In this study, they found evidence of daydreaming and compulsive masturbation in more than 80 percent of the sample, in both childhood and adulthood.

In terms of classical conditioning, Abel and Blanchard (1974) noted that “the repeated pairing of fantasized cues with orgasm results in their acquiring sexually arousing properties.” This is consistent with Bandura’s (1969) find- ing that at least three social-learning variables may be important in linking sexual arousal to deviant fantasy: parental modeling of deviant behavior in blatant fashion; repeated associations between the modeled deviant behavior and a strong positive affective response from the child; and reinforcement of the child’s deviant response (Prentky et al., 1989).

John Campbell, of the FBI’s Behavioral Science Unit, felt that the media pro- motes fantasy. “We’re seeing more serial killers in society because we promote violence through media coverage. The problem with all this attention is that it could have a tendency to foster fantasy—and trigger action” (Davids, 1992, p. 150). Fantasy’s role in the serial murderer’s life is providing “an avenue of escape from a world of hate and rejection” (Hazelwood & Douglas, 1980).

Given a scenario in which a person has developed no real sense of self, no concept of esteem or self-worth, and no meaningful reciprocal relationships with those around him, he or she is likely to see an avenue of escape from hate

50 Real-Life Monsters

and rejection. In this person’s fantasy, he or she may remake the present, the past, and the future. He or she may create a world of acceptance and respect. He or she can enjoy the status of a worthwhile person and be a desirable sex partner. Most important, he or she can call all the shots, write all the lines, and fill in all the blanks. The carte blanche control offered in the world of fantasy is priceless—and addicting.

It appears that many serial killers often retreat into their world of fantasy at some point in their developing pathology. They may delve into sexual, violent, graphic scenarios, and use pornography or detective magazines to assist their creative process. Linked with continued masturbation, retreating within themselves, and in self-imposed isolation, they begin down the path of a murderous obsession. Fantasy is also a logical first step toward a dissociative state, a process that allows the serial killer to leave his stream of consciousness for what is, to him, a better place.

Jeffrey Dahmer had sexual fantasies about corpses and open viscera dur- ing a formative stage in his life (Dietz, 1992b). John Joubert fantasized about strangling and eating his babysitter when he was age 6 or 7, following estab- lished patterns of open masturbation and fantasy about strangling and stabbing young boys in their undershorts (Ressler, 1992, p. 120). Monte Rissell was described by a school principal as always “lost in fantasy” (Ressler, 1992, p. 88). Dodd said, “the more I thought about it, the more exciting the idea of murder sounded. . . . Then I started thinking of torture, castration, and even cannibal- ism” (Schechter, 2003, p. 237). Kemper, Berkowitz, Rogers, Nance, Rader— the list goes on—and the stories of their fantasies are endless and all-inclusive.

Of course, fantasy is not the private domain of serial killers. It is, however, consistent in much of the most heinous offenders’ histories. Even Josef Fritzl, whose crimes included drugging his teenage daughter and then holding her, kidnapped, as a prisoner in the basement of their home while abusing, rap- ing, and impregnating her repeatedly over the course of more than 20 years. Fritzl’s criminal personality was described by a Viennese psychotherapist as created between the ages of two and five, and “the sexual fantasies he admits to having about his mother” set the stage for the inner soul created within him (Hall, 2008, pp. 17–18).

Dr. James T. Reinhardt (1992, p. 208) described the role of fantasy:

By fantasy the murderer attempts to wall himself in against the fatal act, while at the same time qualifying the compulsive psychic demands in the development and use of fantasy. These sadistic fantasies seem always to precede the brutal act of lust murder. These fantasies take all sorts of grotesque and cruel forms. The pervert, on this level of degeneracy, may resort to pornographic pictures, grotesque and cruel literary episodes, out of which he weaves fantasies. On these, his imagination dwells until he loses all contact with reality, only to find himself suddenly impelled to carry out his fantasies into the world of actuality. This is done, appar- ently, by drawing some human objects into the fantasy.

Environment, Background, and Personality 51

The serial killer has often lost himself in a world of fantasy, a world where fantasy is omnipresent and he cannot discern what fantasy is and what reality is. This psychodynamic makes possible the continued execution of violence, sadism, and murder for personal satisfaction. Fantasy can also be seen as a reason the serial killer can calmly and methodically dehumanize his victims. He then reconnects with the real world (Bundy’s live-in girlfriend referred to his “using me to touch base with reality”) and carries on immediately after the crime with such seemingly mundane activities as going out for hamburgers (Kendall, 1981, p. 175).

PORNOGRAPHY AND CAUSATION A logical question, after the subject of fantasy is, what role does pornog-

raphy play? There are those who feel pornography is a causative precursor in the actions of a serial killer. It’s certainly a popular feminist ideal. One FBI study showed that, of the sexual or serial killers surveyed, 81 percent listed pornography as their primary sexual interest. The study also noted that the killers were “characteristically immersed in fantasy,” which supports the prior discussion of fantasy but merely suggests the role of pornography. A North Carolina State Police study found that 75 percent of defendants in violent sex crimes “had some kind of hard-core pornographic material” in their homes or vehicles (Mellish, 1989).

Dietz, Harry, and Hazelwood (1986) have suggested that detective maga- zines contributed to the development of sexual sadism, that they facilitate sadistic fantasies, and that they might serve as training manuals and equip- ment catalogs for sex criminals. Ted Bundy’s claims that he was a victim of pornography, made just before his execution, are well documented. Even Andrei Chikatilo, with pictures of naked women in his holding cell, blamed pornography as the “cause of his troubles.”

What of it? Most researchers say that a correlational relationship does not prove anything, much less cause and effect. It would seem reasonable that persons who are so obsessed with sex and fantasy would have pornography in their homes, much as any sports fan would have Sports Illustrated or The Sporting News . But does pornography or detective magazines incite someone to murder—someone who might not otherwise? Not likely. It should also be noted that Bundy’s and Chikatilo’s credibility in this matter must be consid- ered suspect and their motives unknown. Durham (1986)—in response to feminist assertions that pornography is harmful, incites violence, and should be regulated—noted that “the use of unsupported assertions, the limited gen- eralizability of the social-psychological experimental research, the inability of the research to measure the magnitude of the effects of pornography and the failure to conduct the discussion in comparative terms severely undermines the persuasiveness of the argument” (p. 101).

The matter of pornography deserves a study of its own. It might be believ- able that pornographic addiction could relate to a sexual offender. In consid- ering the needs of a pattern murderer, the person who kills, not just sexually

52 Real-Life Monsters

assaults, there’s a missing link in the argument. It would appear that pornog- raphy is to the serial killer as gasoline is to the arsonist. Both are tools of the sexual criminal. Both are immersed in fantasies and have the motivation to fulfill their erotic desires. Magazines and movies definitely help fuel the fire. However, without gasoline, the arsonist still finds a match. Without the por- nography, Ted Bundy kills scores of women anyway.

Still, included in this book is a personal series of conversations in 2011 with a serial murderer who had some interesting ideas about pornography—some perspectives I hadn’t considered. The subject may need to be revisited. This discussion is found in Chapter 9.

LESSER CRIMES Obsessive fantasizing, it appears, usually results in a feeling-out period, a

time when lesser crimes are committed. Just as in experimentation with fire or animal torture while still a child, the fledgling serial killer usually tries the lesser roles of arson, burglary, theft, sexual deviance, molestation, and assault. Criminal journals are full of Bundys, Dahmers, and DeSalvos who worked their way up the ladder of antisocial acts and behaviors, petty crimes, and control-seizing acts on their way to habitual homicide.

The role of lesser crimes is especially important, as the crimes are usually sexual in nature. These acts—most often arson, breaking and entering, sexual assault, or burglary—represent sexual gratification and control. Although on a smaller scale, these crimes appear to reflect the basic motivations of the serial killer. For example, criminal profiler Diana Sievers (personal communication, 1992) of the Illinois State Police stated that breaking and entering or bur- glary is an example of the individual gaining the control that he seeks.

Even clearly mentally ill or disorganized offenders such as Richard Chase committed earlier crimes such as breaking and entering, defecating on a child’s bed, and urinating on clothing in a drawer—all “signs of classic fetish burglaries” (Ressler, 1992, p. 16).

Russell Williams continued a ritual that included photographing women’s bedrooms including “underwear drawers, wall photos, certificates identifica- tion and oftentimes would take pictures of the undergarments laid out in painstakingly coordinated arrangements” (Gibb, 2011, p. 217). He’d often take the time to strip down and pose for self-portraits and masturbate in their rooms while wearing the women’s underwear.

William Heirens committed his first killings, he says, because the victims walked in on him while he was in the act of committing burglaries described as sexual in nature. Heirens’s murders were categorized by Ressler as a “con- tinuation of the burglaries and other crimes he committed earlier in his teen- age years” (p. 50), and he continued to commit many more in between the murders. Danny Rolling was serving four life sentences in Florida State Prison for a series of armed robberies and home burglaries committed in Gainesville, Ocala, and Tampa, at the time of his conviction for the murders of five college students (Abdo, 1994).

Environment, Background, and Personality 53

Heirens’s and DeSalvo’s countless burglaries, or Berkowitz’s arsons (1,488 in New York—he kept a diary—as well as his pulling several hundred false alarms) are lesser crimes with no motivation other than getting a sexual charge and seizing control.

AN ISSUE OF CONTROL The serial murderer is hard to dissect, even on paper. Factors such as sa-

dism, fear, sexual perversion, emotional maladaptivness, mounting anger, and hostility apply in different degrees to different offenders. The one constant here, however, is control. Control—whether it is sexual, sadistic, revengeful, or any of the other mind-sets held by the killer—seems to be the underlying drive and is the source of the serial killer’s motivation and direction to “pos- sess them . . . forever.” As Levin and Fox (1985, p. 68) have noted,

In large part, the pleasure and exhilaration that the serial killer derives from repeated murder stem from absolute control over other human beings. As Roy Norris admitted . . . “the rape wasn’t the important part, it was the dominance.”

A series of FBI rape typologies used in profiling violent crimes (Holmes & Holmes, 2009, pp. 160–165) includes the definition of the power assertive rap- ist, a psychopath or a sociopath who needs to express personal dominance. This offender type sounds most like the power- and control-seeking rapist involved in many serial killings. However, behaviors also described by power assurance, anger retaliation, and anger excitation (sadistic) rapists, who often display a compulsive pattern, can also fall into this population.

This link between control, rape, and serial murder was also noted by Egger (1985) and West (1987). It was West who pointed out that “it may take only a small increase in the desperation of the assailant or resisting victim to con- vert a violent rape into a murder” (p. 19).

MACDONALD TRIAD COMPONENTS AS CONTROL ISSUES Indeed, from the earliest inklings of the pathology to come, serial mur-

derers seem to deal continuously with issues of control. The elements of the MacDonald triad (enuresis, fire setting, and animal abuse, introduced in Chapter 3) as future predictors of violence are all examples of impulse control issues, whether it be in regard to holding urine, sadism, or pathological sexual expression.

The late enuresis (bed-wetting) in their pasts is a control issue that is hard for them to ignore. Dr. Joseph Michaels, a pioneer in the analysis of the cor- relation between enuresis and murderous behavior in children, explains “Per- sistently enuretic individuals are impelled to act. They feel the urgency of the moment psychologically, as at an earlier date they could not hold their urine” (Ewing, 1990, p. 11). The biological implications of lack of impulse control,

54 Real-Life Monsters

as previously discussed, plus repeated incidence of such poor impulse control reflect this position.

The prevalence of animal abuse also appears to be a combination of sexual or sadistic urges and control and dominance needs. The literature is again replete with stories such as that of Wayne Nance, who demonstrated his su- periority by dropping kittens into an incinerator or skinning them alive. What easier way to act out your control, which is sorely lacking in real life, than by a skinning a cat or mutilating another defenseless creature? The next logical step is to move on to other objects of easy dominance, such as little girls and boys, women, and other potential victims.

Three teenagers arrested for the brutal murder and sexual mutilation of three eight-year-olds had combined ritual and animal abuse as they skinned, cooked, and ate a dog. One of the young killers noted to a priest that he had already committed himself to hell, and he couldn’t change that (Castaneda, 1993a, b).

Keith Jesperson, the “Happy Face Killer,” killed animals as a child by man- ual strangulation—a glimpse into the future of how he would kill his human victims.

A 2003 criminology journal article tried to apply a graduation hypothesis connecting animal abuse to serial murder (Wright & Hensley, 2003), but only succeeded in listing case history details of serial murderers commonly known to have a history of animal abuse. However, it seems logical at least that an early offender motivated by sadism and impulse control problems could prac- tice on a vulnerable animal and later progress to human beings.

This control-centered triad of early crimes includes arson, which is also very common in serial killers’ pasts. Arson or fire setting has distinct sexual features and represents the actions of taking control of the situation, both in fire setting and in pulling false alarms. Shawcross, Berkowitz, and many other serial killers shared this interest. An additional interesting correlation is the connection of power and control pathology and young fire setters, who often act out of frustration, rage, and a sense of powerlessness. Internal power im- balances caused by organic problems such as ADHD as well as external issues such as family dysfunction and abuse (Kolko, 2002) are commonly related to research regarding fire setting behavior in youth.

PATHOLOGY AND CONTROL Control seems to be the underlying motivation for the personality types

discussed in Chapter 2—the antisocial personality, the psychopath, the socio- path, the borderline personality, the attachment disordered, and the narcissis- tic. They all crave control, whether it is to build a fragile ego or to demonstrate how weak everyone else is.

Even necrophiles acknowledge the importance of control. In a clinical analysis, when asked what their primary motivation for necrophilia was, the subjects often said “total acceptance of the subject [victim] . . . the completely unconditional positive regard and total acceptance” (Meloy, 1993).

Environment, Background, and Personality 55

Erich Fromm (1973), in considering necrophilia, postulated that there is a natural development from the anal, control-obsessed personality, to the sa- distic character, and finally to the necrophilous character. He felt that this development was determined by an increase in narcissism, unrelatedness, and destructiveness, and that necrophilia can be described as “the malignant form of the anal character” (p. 349). He also said that the relatively small number of necrophiles were severely pathological cases and one “should look for a genetic predisposition” for this (p. 367). Dr. Ashok Bedi, after examining Jef- frey Dahmer, agreed with the connection between necrophilia and control. In regard to Dahmer’s necrophilia, he commented:

His personality disorder, the alcohol addiction, the pedophilia, the necrophilia, his ego and homosexuality, all were layers of his dysfunc- tion. The preoccupation of having sex with the dead person had to do with his feelings at different points in time. A living person could not fill his need. Only a dead person, where he was totally in control and there was no way of retaliation, belittlement, negation or abandonment. It was not so much being sadistic as wanting to be in control. Then it became a question of what he was looking for—nurturing, wanting to be fed, that sort of thing. If there was a need to show anger, then he might use anal sex. It depended on what his needs were in his head at the moment.

(Davis, 1991, p. 264)

When considering the issues of control and sadism, J. A. Apsche (1993, p. 117) noted:

When, in children, control is substituted for intimacy [as manipulation and control substituted for closeness], the child mistakenly learns that control is a substitute for intimacy. For the sexual sadist, there is a need to take control to demonstrate power and virility. The mode of death selected is one which indicates that the victim had meaning for the killer, and that the intimacy in the murderous act is part of the close bond between the mur- derer and the victim formed in the killer’s fantasies and delusions.

Erich Fromm, in searching for the nature of sadism, stated that there were different forms of sadism, and they were not independent of one other. In observing what he called the common element, he wrote:

The core of sadism, common to all its manifestations, is the passion to have absolute and unrestricted control over a living being, whether an animal, a child, a man, or a woman. To force someone to endure pain or humiliation without being able to defend himself is one of the manifestations of absolute control, but is by no means the only one. . . .

56 Real-Life Monsters

Complete control over another human being means crippling him, choking him, thwarting him. . . . The person who has complete control over another living being makes this being into his thing, his property, while he becomes the other being’s god.

(1973, p. 289)

Edmund Kemper summed it up: “I wanted the girls for myself as posses- sions. They were going to be mine; they are mine” (Cheney, 1992, p. 171).

THE FIRST KILL The first kill is often the clumsy, inexperienced, and impulsive act of a

virgin to homicide. Reminiscent of the adolescent fumbling in a car’s back seat, David Berkowitz made a botched attempt at stabbing his initial victim. It caused him to eventually change to an easier and more efficient method of shooting his prey.

Sometimes the first kill is the result of what began as a rape or assault, but crossed the line as the frenzy of the episode unfolded. The excitement of the moment made the killer cross the threshold separating fantasy from reality. In other instances, a stressful event in the killer’s life triggers an escalation of activity, from crimes such as burglary or rape, to murder. Some offenders may even develop a pattern of increased homicides coinciding with peaks of stress in their personal lives, acting out in a manner that provides a pattern that can be observed in a context of life stress looking back.

Occasionally, killers will say that they do not even remember the details of their first kill, while showing extraordinary memory and clarity for subse- quent conquests. Their initiation to murder is often followed with guilt, fear, and reflection, similar to the aforementioned adolescent’s first time with sex. The shame and acknowledgment that a grave crime has been committed is felt along with the excitement and the rush that comes with the realization of having finally discovered what they truly need. This is also a time to enjoy mental reenactments. As time passes and fear of reprisal fades, these offenders enjoy renewed confidence in their ability to practice their habit undetected. The initial cooling-off period is reinforced within the cycle, and will become part of the dynamics of habits to come.

THE COMPULSION AND RITUAL OF SERIAL MURDER The completion of the cycle, culminating in the murders, accompanying

sexual activity, and post-offense behavior, illustrates the killer’s obsessive- compulsive behavior and ritualistic actions.

Albert DeSalvo spoke of the indescribable compulsion that came over him when he strangled (Frank, 1967, p. 378). Increased anxiety about and dis- comfort with the compelling urge to kill becomes consuming. The eventual killing provides the expected release and satiation, dissipating the tension and introducing momentary relief. Like the addict, the killer will search for the

Environment, Background, and Personality 57

same level of high as experienced during the first kill, but may settle for the release of built-up anxiety. According to the FBI’s Robert Hazelwood, “Se- rial offenders generally commit their crimes as stress is building on them. The stress level reaches a peak, and that’s when they make the kill. It’s like a weight being lifted off their shoulders” (Coston, 1992, p. 278).

This cycle of anxiety, discomfort, urge, act, and cooling off reflects a similar sequence of emotions and actions cycling within those suffering from OCD or sexual paraphilic obsessions and compulsions, such as exhibitionism. In this case, the urge to kill is obsessive-compulsive.

The times between killings may vary, according to a number of factors. Sometimes there are no victims to meet the killer’s criteria, sexually or oth- erwise. For example, Andrei Chikatilo’s killings dropped off in the winter months owing to cold weather. Jeffrey Dahmer reportedly refrained from killing a couple of young victims because he didn’t have enough time to go to work. Joel Rifkin went months without killing because apparently “examin- ing trophies—the credit cards, panties and jewelry of previous victims—may have been enough to satisfy him” (Eftimiates, 1993, p. 234). Dennis Rader took years off from killing, only to return to his old compulsion, desiring the attentions of media and law enforcement.

However, the obsession remains and generally does not stay dormant for long. When one of Westley Dodd’s young victims asked him, “Why are you doing this to us?” he responded: “Because I have to do it” (King, 1993, p. 288). An example even more illustrative of this compulsion comes from of Ted Bundy:

I have a sickness . . . a disease like your alcoholism. You can’t take an- other drink and with my sickness. . . . There is something . . . that I just can’t be around. . . . I know it now. . . . There is something the matter with me. . . . I just couldn’t contain it. I’ve fought it for a long, long time. . . . It got too strong. I tried, believe me, to suppress it. That’s why I didn’t do well in school. My time was being used trying to make my life look normal. But it wasn’t normal. All the time I could feel the force building in me.

(Kendall, 1981, pp. 174, 176)

Bundy’s live-in girlfriend, under her pen name Elizabeth Kendall, appreci- ated the comparison to her own pattern of alcoholism:

I didn’t pretend to understand or accept Ted’s compulsion to kill beauti- ful, vital young women. But I do understand something of compulsion, and I do understand something of what it feels like to repeat compulsive actions over and over again, even though the intention is to never do it again. In my case it was getting drunk repeatedly when I didn’t want to. In Ted’s case it was so much worse.

(1981, p. 177)

58 Real-Life Monsters

At this point it would appear that the killer settles into a pattern of cyclical murderous acts, becoming more and more comfortable in his practices, also gaining efficiency and skill in technique and execution. Still, the compulsion and ritual of the kill retains its importance. Wilson and Seaman (1991) ob- served in regard to Jack the Ripper, “the ritual was of supreme importance to the Ripper. More than that, it was a clamorous, overpowering need, a compulsion, which overruled all other considerations that night—personal safety included” (p. 37). They also noted, regarding an 1809 female killer, Anna Zwanziger, that “as if the need to kill was an addiction, Zwanziger told the judge it would have been impossible for her to cease poisoning others and described the virulent poison as her ‘truest friend’ ” (p. 49).

Ritual was clearly important to Ed Gein, the inspiration for serial killer characters in several movies, who:

robbed corpses and body parts from a number of graves. Gein used these limbs and organs to fashion ornaments, such as a belt of nipples and a hanging human head, as well as decorations for his house, includ- ing chairs upholstered with human skin and bed posts crowned with skulls. A shoe box containing nine vulvas was but one part of Gein’s col- lection of female organs. On moonlit evenings he would prance around his farm wearing a real female mask, a vest of skin complete with female breasts, and women’s panties filled with vaginas in an attempt to recre- ate the form and presence of his dead mother.

(Levin & Fox, 1985, p. 4)

Part of the ritual often includes keeping diaries, photo albums, and other visual aids for reenactment of the crimes. Westley Dodd kept a graphic diary of his activities and plans along with a categorically detailed photo album, in- cluding descriptions of experiments to perform on the dead (and the living), murder methods, and plans for creating a torture rack.

Trophies were of course as important as the ritual. Immediately after the strangulation of one victim, Russell Williams couldn’t resist:

Her killer’s first impulse was to run and grab his camera. He took three photographs of her body as it lay on the floor, taking care to pose his murderous flashlight next to her, as though she were a hunter’s trophy kill. He then cut the zip tie from around her neck to keep as a souvenir.

(Gibb, 2011, p. 98)

Another example: Robert Berdella, a sadistic rapist and murderer of six, kept skulls, an envelope of teeth, and hundreds of pictures of young

Environment, Background, and Personality 59

men being raped and tortured. He cut up the bodies to be put out with the trash. An excerpt from his diary included this page (Jackman & Cole, 1992, p. 97):

7:30 Bar Ferris [victim] 9/26 Drug 9:00 Out 9:05 Shoes + socks off, move arms snoring no rea 9:10 Test need no react [needle] 2 1/2 cp left a “3 cp

right a” 9:20 Photo, clothes off, no react 9:40 Turned over, slight arm movement 9:50 Fing F no reac [finger sex] 1 1/2 cc ket arm no rea

Front F no react 10:15 BF no reac [anal sex] 10:30 tied arms 10:50–11:00 Carrot F Slight resist 1 1/2 cc cp nk 11:00 2 cc cp vein 11:30–11:45 BF, cub F, slight react Regag 12:00 Fightin

The notes continued for two more pages. The top of the third page was marked “Fri,” and the last two entries read:

11:45 Very delayed breathing, snoring 12:00 86

The kills become increasingly intense, lessening the effect of a “fix” for this addict of death. Also experiencing shorter cooling-off periods, the ob- sessed killer may get sloppy (e.g., Dahmer, Bianchi, Rifkin) or more frenzied (Bundy) as time goes on. Some killers, such as Kemper and DeSalvo, say they reach the point when they could have stopped. This might seem plausible for a Kemper, who finally destroyed the object of his rage (his mother), but it is highly unlikely for seasoned serial killers to walk away from their compulsive patterns. Indeed, frenzy, compulsion, sadism, and ritual were all reflected in the words of one of history’s first serial killers, Jack the Ripper:

I left nothing of the bitch, nothing. I placed it all over the room, time was on my hands, like the other whore I cut the bitches nose all of it this time. I left nothing of her face to remember her by. . . . I thought it a joke when I cut her breasts off, kissed them for awhile. The taste of blood was sweet, the pleasure was overwhelming, will have to do it again. It thrilled me so. Left them on the table with some of the other stuff. Thought they belonged there. They wanted a slaughterman so

60 Real-Life Monsters

I stripped what I could, laughed while I was doing so. Like the other bitches she ripped like a ripe peach. . . . One of these days I will take the head with me, I will boil it and serve it up for my supper.

(Harrison & Barrett, 1993, p. 102)

Occasionally, a serial killer’s run comes to an end purely by chance, such as with a routine traffic stop, in the case of Ted Bundy. In New York, Joel Rifkin, who was pulled over for running a stop sign, was discovered carrying a woman’s decomposing body in the rear of his truck. He quickly confessed to the killing of 17 women (“Man reportedly,” 1993).

In the vast majority of the cases, however, serial killers will kill until they are caught, by law enforcement efforts, through their own self-destructive misstep, or when they meet their own death. This cheery observation is tem- pered, however, by the fact that we can never know how many multiple mur- derers might have killed, then ceased on their own for whatever reason and were never apprehended. It is rare indeed to have an Edmund Kemper come to the end of the line and turn himself in, but most likely not as rare as a sea- soned killer who simply opted to retire from a compulsion that drove him his entire adult life.

Part II Part II

Toward an Examination of a Theory of Violence

This page intentionally left blank

Theoretical Discussion and Methodolog y

5

I will not allow too much time before my next. Indeed, I need to repeat my pleasure as soon as possible.

—Jack the Ripper

PRELIMINARY IMPRESSIONS In suggesting a pattern for the synthesis of the serial killer, the literature makes reference to many phenomena: the incidence of mental, physical, and sexual abuse; the possibility of organic or biological damage or predisposition; prenatal stress; and the development of mental and attitude maladjustments as well as sexual dysfunctions. With consideration of the potential of vari- ables, the result appears to be an inclusive dynamic: multiple factors acting in concert. The serial murderer’s personality develops through a combination of specific variables that feed on themselves, creating a feedback loop for the activity.

Of course, there are many competing varied theories for the most extremely violent criminals. However, most do tend to take a stance favoring heavily on one side or the other of nature versus nurture. Some will lean almost exclu- sively on nature, which is very difficult to scientifically prove, whereas oth- ers put far more weight on the environment. Athens (1992) argued it was a combination of social experiences, including measurable stages (brutalization, belligerency, and violent performance). These stages had to be completed by

64 Real-Life Monsters

meeting specific criteria—for example, brutalization had to be experienced by violent subjugation, personal horrification, and violent coaching. While these would be considered common in the creation of many violent criminals, they do not apply consistently to the serial murderer. These features would easily fit a Henry Lee Lucas, but would be very difficult to apply to Jeffrey Dahmer, acknowledging the difficulty in the completeness and accuracy of case histo- ries. The study of the serial murderer still suggests a theory that includes the interaction of the physical and the social.

The concept of an interactional dynamic is not without precedent. Eric W. Hickey (1997) suggested an interactional, socio-developmental model he called trauma control, emphasizing the importance of social structure and stressing the influence of a traumatic event, or triggering mechanism. The interactional dynamic presented here is similar, but the key element is a bio- logical component, which is at least equal to environmental factors.

THE DIATHESIS–STRESS MODEL The theory proposed in this book is a diathesis–stress model, a systems ap-

proach similar to what is currently offered in the psychological literature for conditions such as schizophrenia. There certainly is a social, environmental factor—or trigger; most obviously it is the extreme psychological trauma suf- fered by the vast majority of serial killers. This alone, however, is not enough. There seems to be an additional component—a physiological, biological in- gredient that makes the mix an explosive one. Simply put, the diathesis–stress model combines both nature and nurture. Neither a biological factor nor an environmental situation alone seem to be enough to produce the offender type in question. A biological predisposition acts in concert with a traumatic environmental situation.

The diathesis–stress (diathesis: biological; stress: environmental pressure) model of schizophrenia was introduced by Meehl in 1962 (cited in Ingram & Price, 2010, p. 26), which “helped to integrate the biological and [emphasis added] environmental models of psychopathology.” It was further postulated by Gottesman and Shields in 1982. The term refers to a genetic predisposi- tion being a necessary condition, but not sufficient in itself (Lewine, Gulley, Risch, Jewart, & Houpt, 1990). Hans and Marcus (1987) studied a process model for schizophrenia in diathesis–stress terms, underlining the importance of factors such as constitutional vulnerability—that is, biological and genetic risk via family history; early neurobehavioral signs marking a constitutional defect; stressful childhood environment, particularly family environment; and early premorbid childhood signs of poor social adjustment. Their model for etiology “included a constitutional, possibly genetic vulnerability, aggravated by environmental, most likely familial, stresses.” It is strikingly similar to the histories of many serial killers and forms a reasonable theoretical position for this particular psychodynamic.

In Vulnerability to Psychopathology, Ingram and Price explain that the use of diathesis–stress concept within medical terminology dates back to ancient

Theoretical Discussion and Methodolog y 65

Greeks and “was well-lodged in the psychiatric vernacular” by the late 1800s. Furthermore, diathesis:

Signifies a predisposition to illness and has evolved from its original focus on constitutional, biological factors to presently also encompass psyc

"Order a similar paper and get 15% discount on your first order with us
Use the following coupon
"FIRST15"

Order Now