Category Model

Category Model

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 1991. “WJI. 61, No. 2, 226-244

Copyright 1991 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0022-3514/91/S3.00

Attachment Styles Among \bung Adults: A Test of a Four-Category Model

Kim Bartholomew Simon Eraser University

Burnaby, British Columbia, Canada

Leonard M. Horowitz Stanford University

A new 4-group model of attachment styles in adulthood is proposed. Four prototypic attachment patterns are defined using combinations of a person’s self-image (positive or negative) and image of others (positive or negative). In Study 1, an interview was developed to yield continuous and categori- cal ratings of the 4 attachment styles. Intercorrelations of the attachment ratings were consistent with the proposed model. Attachment ratings were validated by self-report measures of self-con- cept and interpersonal functioning. Each style was associated with a distinct profile of interper- sonal problems, according to both self- and friend-reports. In Study 2, attachment styles within the family of origin and with peers were assessed independently. Results of Study I were replicated. The proposed model was shown to be applicable to representations of family relations; Ss’ attachment styles with peers were correlated with family attachment ratings.

This article describes a new model of attachment styles in adulthood. Drawing on the theory of Bowlby (1973, 1980, 1982a), two types of internal working models are postulated— an internal model of the self and an internal model of others. Each internal model can be dichotomized as positive or nega- tive to yield four theoretical attachment styles. This article sum- marizes the relevant childhood attachment literature, reviews recent work on adult attachment, describes the new model, and then presents two empirical studies designed to validate the proposed model.

Chi ldhood Attachment and Internal Models

Attachment theory conceptualizes “the propensity of human beings to make strong affectional bonds to particular others” (Bowlby, 1977, p. 201). Bowlby hypothesizes that an attachment system evolved to maintain proximity between infants and their caretakers under conditions of danger or threat. More recent formulations view the attachment system as functioning continuously to provide children with a sense of “felt security” which facilitates exploration by the child (Ainsworth, Blehar, Waters, & Wall, 1978; Sroufe & Waters, 1977). The quality of early attachment relationships is thus rooted in the degree to which the infant has come to rely on the attachment figure as a source of security (Ainsworth et al., 1978).

On the basis of infants’ responses to separation from and

This article is based on Kim Bartholomew’s doctoral dissertation at Stanford University

Preparation of this article was supported by Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada doctoral and postdoctoral fellowships to Kim Bartholomew.

We thank James Fuendeling for helping to develop and conduct in- terviews and Diana Doumas, Christine Duffy, Mike Emerzian, Gail Halloway, Stephanie Kondik, and Suzanne Little for coding inter- views.

Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Kim Bartholomew, Department of Psychology, Simon Fraser University, Burnaby, British Columbia, Canada V5A 1S6.

reunion with caretakers in a structured laboratory procedure, Ainsworth identified three distinct patterns of infant attach- ment: secure, anxious-resistant, and avoidant. Children classi- fied as securely attached welcome their caretaker’s return after a separation and, if distressed, seek proximity and are readily comforted. Infants classified as anxious-resistant show ambiva- lent behavior toward caregivers and an inability to be com- forted on reunion. Infants classified as avoidant avoid proxim- ity or interaction with the caretaker on reunion. Continuity in infant attachment patterns seems to be mediated largely by continuity in the quality of primary attachment relationships (see Lamb, Thompson, Gardner, Charnov, & Estes, 1985).

According to Bowlby’s theory, children, over time, internalize experiences with caretakers in such a way that early attachment relations come to form a prototype for later relationships out- side the family. Bowlby (1973) identifies two key features of these internal representations or working models of attachment: “(a) whether or not the attachment figure is judged to be the sort of person who in general responds to calls for support and protection; [and] (b) whether or not the self is judged to be the sort of person towards whom\anyone, and the attachment fig- ure in particular, is likely to respond in a helpful way” (p. 204). The first concerns the child’s image of other people; the second concerns the child’s image of the self. Recent research has exam- ined the nature of internal working models in relation to chil- dren’s earlier attachment styles. The data show, for example, that children classified as ambivalent hold negative views of themselves, but the data are not as consistent with respect to children classified as avoidant (Cassidy, 1988; Kaplan & Main, 1985; Main, Kaplan, & Cassidy, 1985). A considerable body of research also links the child’s attachment style at 12 or 18 months to the child’s social and emotional adjustment through early childhood (see Bretherton, 1985).

Attachment in Adulthood

A basic principle of attachment theory is that attachment relationships continue to be important throughout the life span (Ainsworth, 1982,1989; Bowlby, 1977,1980,1982b). Although

226

ATTACHMENT STYLES 227

evidence exists documenting the continuity of attachment-re- lated behaviors (see Belsky & Pensky, 1988; Bowlby, 1973,1980; Ricks, 1985; Rutter, 1988), investigators have only recently ex- amined the relationship between working models of attach- ment and social and emotional adaptation in adults. Main has developed an Adult Attachment Interview (George, Kaplan, & Main, 1987; Main & Goldwyn, 1988) that explores adults* repre- sentations of childhood attachment relations. On the basis of these interviews, mothers have been classified into attachment groups that parallel the three childhood attachment patterns described above and are predictive of the quality of the mother’s interaction with her own child and the security of the child’s attachment (Crowell & Feldman, 1987; Grossmann, Fremmer- Bombik, Rudolf, & Grossmann, 1988; Main et al, 1985). Ko- bak and Sceery (1988) used this procedure to examine young adults’ self- and other-representations, providing some evidence that secure subjects view themselves as relatively undistressed and others as supportive, that dismissive (avoidant) subjects view the self as undistressed and others as unsupportive, and that preoccupied subjects (corresponding to anxious-resistant children) view the self as distressed and others as supportive. (They did not discuss a category of people who might exhibit a negative view of both the self and others.)

Hazan and Shaver (1987) conceptualized romantic love as an attachment process and developed a self-report procedure to classify adults into three categories that correspond to the three attachment styles of childhood. In contrast to Main’s proce- dure, these investigators relied on respondents’ self-reports rather than on inferences from a semi-structured interview. Their results showed that compared with the secure group, the two insecure groups reported more negative experiences and beliefs about love, had a history of shorter romantic relation- ships, and provided less favorable descriptions of their child- hood relationships with parents (see also Collins & Read, 1990). Subjects in the two insecure groups also reported more self- doubt and less acceptability to others than did those endorsing a secure self-description (see also Feeney & Noller, 1990).

These two approaches differed both in the particular attach- ment relationships focused on (parent-child versus love rela- tionships) and in the methodology used for classifying subjects (interview versus self-report). Whereas the interview method identified avoidant adults as people who denied experiencing subjective distress and downplayed the importance of attach- ment needs, the self-report method identified people who re- ported feeling subjective distress and discomfort when they be- come close to others. Thus, a single avoidant-detached category may obscure conceptually separable patterns of avoidance in adulthood. Moreover, although Bowlby (1973) suggested that working models differ in terms of images of self and others, no study has considered all four categories that are logically de- rived by combining the two levels of self-image (positive vs. negative) with the two levels of image of others (positive vs. negative). The present research examined all four of these cate- gories and assessed subjects through an interview as well as through subjects’ own self-reports.

A Model of Adult Attachment

The model of the self and the model of the other as conceptu- alized by Bowlby can be combined to describe prototy pic forms

of adult attachment (Bartholomew, 1990). If a person’s abstract image of the self is dichotomized as positive or negative (the self as worthy of love and support or not) and if the person’s ab- stracted image of the other is also dichotomized as positive or negative (other people are seen as trustworthy and available vs. unreliable and rejecting), then four combinations can be con- ceptualized. Figure 1 shows the four attachment patterns that are derived from a combination of the two dimensions. Each cell represents a theoretical ideal, or prototype (Cantor, Smith, French, & Mezzich, 1980; Horowitz, Wright, Lowenstein, & Parad, 1981; Rosen, 1978), that different people might approxi- mate to different degrees.

Cell I indicates a sense of worthiness (lovability) plus an ex- pectation that other people are generally accepting and respon- sive. Because this cell corresponds conceptually to categories that investigators call securely attached (e.g., Hazan & Shaver, 1987; Main et al, 1985), we labeled it secure. Cell II indicates a sense of unworthiness (unlovability) combined with a positive evaluation of others. This combination of characteristics would lead the person to strive for self-acceptance by gaining the ac- ceptance of valued others. This pattern corresponds concep- tually to Hazan and Shaver’s ambivalent group (Hazan & Shaver, 1987) and to Main’s enmeshed or preoccupied with at- tachment pattern (Main et al., 1985) and is referred to as preoc- cupied. Cell III indicates a sense of unworthiness (unlovability) combined with an expectation that others will be negatively disposed (untrustworthy and rejecting). By avoiding close in- volvement with others, this style enables people to protect themselves against anticipated rejection by others. Although not explicitly discussed in previous work in adult attachment, this style may correspond in part to the avoidant style described by Hazan and Shaver (1987). We therefore labeled it fearful- avoidant. Finally, Cell IV indicates a sense of love-worthiness combined with a negative disposition toward other people. Such people protect themselves against disappointment by avoiding close relationships and maintaining a sense of inde- pendence and invulnerability. This style corresponds concep- tually to the detached or dismissing of attachment attitude de- scribed by Main et al. (1985), so we labeled it dismissive- avoidant.

The dimensions in Figure 1 can also be conceptualized in

MODEL OF SELF (Dependence)

Positive (Low)

Negative (High)

Positive (Low)

MODELOF OTHER (Avoidance)

Negative (High)

CELL I

SECURE Comfortable with

intimacy and autonomy

CELL IV

DISMISSING Dismissing of intimacy

Counter-dependent

CELL II

PREOCCUPIED Preoccupied with

relationships

CELL HI

FEARFUL Fearful of intimacy Socially avoidanl

Figure 1. Model of adult attachment.

228 KIM BARTHOLOMEW AND LEONARD M. HOROWITZ

terms of dependency on the horizontal axis and the avoidance of intimacy on the vertical axis (see labels in parentheses). De- pendency can vary from low (a positive self-regard is estab- lished internally and does not require external validation) to high (positive self-regard can only be maintained by others’ on- going acceptance). Avoidance of intimacy reflects the degree to which people avoid close contact with others as a result of their expectations of aversive consequences. The dismissing and fearful styles are alike in that both reflect the avoidance of intimacy; they differ, however, in the person’s need for others’ acceptance to maintain a positive self-regard. Similarly, the preoccupied and fearful groups are alike in that both exhibit strong dependency on others to maintain a positive self-regard, but they differ in their readiness to become involved in close relationships. Whereas the preoccupied cell implies a reaching out to others in an attempt to fulfill dependency needs, the fearful cell implies an avoidance of closeness to minimize even- tual disappointment. Therefore, cells in adjoining quadrants of Figure 1 are more similar conceptually than those in opposite quadrants.

Study 1

We administered a semi-structured interview asking subjects to describe their friendship patterns; the subjects’ responses were used to assess the degree to which each person approxi- mated each of the four styles in Figure 1. We also obtained self-report and friends’ ratings of each attachment style, so the three sources of data could be compared. Additional question- naires were administered to subjects and their friends to test hypotheses implied by the model. In particular, self-reports of self-concept, sociability, and interpersonal problems were ob- tained, and self-reports of interpersonal problems were corrob- orated by the judgments of close friends.

The study tested three specific hypotheses. First, multidi- mensional scalings of each set of ratings (by raters, subjects, and friends) were expected to reproduce the organization of Figure 1 across all three sources of data (interview, self-report, and friend-report). Second, self-concept measures were expected to differentiate groups with a positive model of the self (secure and dismissing) from those with a negative model of the self (preoc- cupied and fearful), whereas a sociability measure was expected to differentiate groups with a positive model of others (secure and preoccupied) from those with a negative model of others (fearful and dismissing). Third, the groups were expected to differ from each other in their interpersonal problems. The groups with a negative image of self (preoccupied and fearful) were expected to exhibit problems with passivity and unasserti- veness, whereas those with a negative image of others (fearful and dismissing) were expected to describe problems with socia- lizing and intimacy Problems described by the secure group were not expected to be distinctive in content,

Method

Subjects

Forty female and 37 male students from an introductory psychology class constituted the target sample. They ranged in age from 18 to 22 (M = 19.6); 67% were White, 16% Asian, 5% Hispanic, 8% Black, and

4% other. An equal number of same-sex friends constituted the friend sample. The friends’ age ranged from 18 to 23 (M = 19.8); 65% were White, 13% Asian, 13% Hispanic, 4% Black, and 5% other.

Procedure

Equal numbers of men and women were randomly selected from the subject pool and contacted by telephone. First-year undergraduate stu- dents were excluded to ensure that subjects had been at college long enough to make close friends. Potential subjects were invited to partici- pate with a friend in “a study of friendship and how well people know each other.” Friends were required to be close, same-sex, nonromantic friends whom subjects had known for at least 6 months. Subjects re- ceived course credit for participation, and their accompanying friends were paid $5.

Subjects were tested in groups of two to four friendship pairs. Sub- jects and their friends completed two sets of questionnaires, one re- questing demographic and personality information about themselves and the other asking the same questions of their partner. Instructions were given for participants to answer the second set of questions “ac- cording to your perceptions and knowledge of your friend’s character, feelings or behavior, and not according to how you think your friend may be likely to answer them.” Then each subject and friend were separated to complete the questionnaires. To avoid contrast effects, target subjects completed the self-report questionnaires first, and their friend completed the friend-report first. A second session was then scheduled for subjects to receive the interview.

Measures

Attachment interview. The first author administered a semi-struc- tured interview, which lasted approximately 60 min, to subjects in the target sample. Each interview was tape recorded. The interviewer asked subjects to describe their friendships, romantic relationships, and feelings about the importance of close relationships. If subjects had not been involved in a romantic relationship, they were asked the rea- sons. They were asked about loneliness, shyness, their degreeof trust of others, their impressions of other people’s evaluations of themselves, and their hopes for any changes in their social lives.

On the basis of the interview audio recordings, three raters indepen- dently rated each subject on four 9-point scales describing the subject’s degree of correspondence with each of the four prototypes. The raters comprised two advanced female undergraduate psychology majors and one female graduate student; they were blind to all other measures in the study. A set of criteria (available from the first author) described each prototype, and the rater was instructed to judge how well a sub- ject’s responses matched each of the prototypic descriptions. The four prototypes can be briefly summarized as follows. The secure prototype is characterized by a valuing of intimate friendships, the capacity to maintain close relationships without losing personal autonomy, and a coherence and thoughtfulness in discussing relationships and related issues. The dismissing prototype is characterized by a downplaying of the importance of close relationships, restricted emotionality, an em- phasis on independence and self-reliance, and a lack of clarity or credi- bility in discussing relationships. The preoccupiedprototype is charac- terized by an overinvolvement in close relationships, a dependence on other people’s acceptance fora sense of personal well-being, a tendency to idealize other people, and incoherence and exaggerated emotiona- lity in discussing relationships. The fearful prototype is characterized by an avoidance of close relationships because of a fear of rejection, a sense of personal insecurity, and a distrust of others. Alpha coefficients were computed to assess the reliability of the prototype ratings. The reliabilities ranged from .87 to .95. The ratings were averaged, and the highest of the four average ratings was considered to be the best-fitting

ATTACHMENT STYLES 229

category for that subject. From this procedure, 47% of the sample was classified as secure, 18% as dismissing, 14% as preoccupied, and 21 % as fearful. In addition, the raters were asked to rate each interview along 15 dimensions of relevance to adult attachment (see Appendix A for definitions of the dimensions).

Self- and friend-reports. All subjects completed demographics and friendship questionnaires, two self-concept measures, and a sociability measure. In addition, the subjects completed the Relationship Ques- tionnaire and the Inventory of Interpersonal Problems twice, once to describe themselves and once to describe their friend. The friend’s version of these questionnaires was identical to the standard self-re- port version, with the exception of wording: Instead of “I try to please other people too much,” for example, the friend’s version read “F[your friend ] tries to please other people too much.” The following question- naires were used:

1. The Demographics Questionnaire included family information (e.g., marital status of parents, number of siblings) and personal activi- ties (e.g., exercise, religious observance). Seven-point items assessed the degree of experienced depression, anxiety, and happiness (with re- versed scoring). These three items were combined into a composite measure of subjective distress ifx = .68).

2. The Friendship Questionnaire contained equivalent demographic, factual, and personal questions about the friend. One item assessed the duration of the friendship, and five items assessed the nature of the friendship (e.g., “Compared with close friendships you’ve had in the past, how close is your friendship with FT*). The latter five items were combined into a friendship closeness scale (target sample a = .80; friend sample a = .86).

3. The Rosenberg Self-Esteem Inventory (Rosenberg, 1965) is a 10- item scale that measures global self-esteem. A sample item is “I cer- tainly feel useless at times” (reverse scored; coefficient a = .85).

4. The Fey Self-Acceptance Scale(Fey, 1955) is a 20-item measure of self-acceptance. A representative item is “I’m pretty satisfied with the way I am” (coefficient a = .86).

5. The Sociability Scale (Cheek & Buss, 1981) is a 5-item measure that assesses the degree to which people like to socialize with others. A sample item is “I like to be with people” (self-report coefficient a = .74; friend-report coefficient = .78).

6. The Relationship Questionnaire is an adaptation of the attach- ment measure developed by Hazan and Shaver (1987). This measure consists of four short paragraphs describing the four attachment styles (see Appendix B). Each respondent is asked to make ratings on a 7- point scale of the degree to which they (or their friend) resemble each of the four styles. These ratings are referred to as the self-report and friend-report attachment ratings.

7. The Inventory of Interpersonal Problems (IIP; Horowitz, Rosen- berg, Baer, Ureno, & Vitlasenor, 1988; Horowitz, Rosenberg, Ureno, Kalehzan, & O’Halloran, 1989) is a 127-item inventory designed to assess interpersonal difficulties in a broad cross-section of interper- sonal domains. Subjects are asked to describe the amount of distress that they have experienced from each interpersonal problem on a 5- point scale ranging from not at all (0) to extremely (4). The IIP has demonstrated acceptable reliability, validity, and sensitivity to changes that occur during psychotherapy (Horowitz et al., 1988; 1989). Re- sponses on this inventory were scored using a circumplex. procedure that locates subjects’ responses within a two-dimensional interper- sonal space (defined by the dimensions of warmth and dominance) that can be divided into eight octants (Kiesler, 1983; Wiggins, 1979). Sepa- rate subscales describe distress in each of these octants (Alden, Wig- gins, & Pincus, 1990). Starting with the region of greatest dominance (neutral on warmth) and moving counterclockwise, the eight octants are labeled overly autocratic (e.g., “I try to control other people too much”), overly competitive (e.g., “I fight with other people too much”), overly cold (e.g., “I keep other people at a distance too much”), overly

introverted (e.g., “I feel embarrassed in front of other people too much”), overly subassertive (e.g., “It is hard for me to be assertive with another person”), overly exploitable (e.g., “I let other people take advantage of me too much”), overly nurturant (e.g., “I put other people’s needs before my own too much”), and overly expressive (e.g., “I want to be noticed too much”). (Self-report subscales coefficient a = .66 to .88; friend-report subscales = .71 to .89.)

Results

Description of Attachment Groups

The 15 dimensions rated in the attachment interview were used to identify a profile of correlates for each of the four at- tachment prototypes. Because the prototype ratings were based, in part, on subjects’ dimension scores, these data should be regarded as primarily descriptive. Table 1 shows mean rat- ings on each of the 15 dimensions rated in the attachment inter- view for subjects classified according to their highest prototype rating, as well as the results of one-way analyses of variance and Newman-Keuls comparisons for each dimension. Significant group differences existed for each of the 15 dimensions. Corre- lations between each dimension and each of the four continu- ous prototype ratings were also calculated.

As shown in Table 1, the secure group obtained uniquely high ratings on the coherence of their interviews and the degree of intimacy of their friendships. They also received high ratings on warmth, balance of control in friendships, and level of involve- ment in romantic relationships. Correspondingly; the secure prototype correlated highly with ratings of coherence, r(75) = .78, p < .001, intimacy, r(75) = .77, p < .001, balance of control in friendships, r(15) = .29, p < .01, level of involvement in romantic relationships, r{15) = .40, p < .001, self-confidence, r(75) = .41, p < .001, and warmth, r(75) = .59, p < .001.

The dismissing group scored uniquely high on self-confi- dence and uniquely low on emotional expressiveness, frequency of crying, and warmth. They scored lower than the secure and preoccupied on all scales reflecting closeness in personal rela- tionships: self-disclosure, intimacy, level of romantic involve- ments, capacity to rely on others, and use of others as a secure base. They were also rated as being low on elaboration and caregiving and as being more in control than their social partners in both friendships and romantic relationships. Con- tinuous ratings of the dismissing prototype correspondingly correlated positively with self-confidence, r(75) = .52, p < .001, and negatively with elaboration, r{15) = – .43, p < .001, emo- tional expressiveness, r{15) = -.69, p < .001, frequency of cry- ing, r(75) = -.55, p < .001, warmth, r(75) = -.68, p < .001, caregiving, r(75) = -.37, p < .001, and with all scales signifying involvement in close relationships—self-disclosure, r{75) = -.45, p < .001, intimacy, r(15) = -.33, p < .01, level of romantic involvements, r{15) = -.35, p < .001, reliance on others, r(15) = -.57, p < .001, and use of others as a secure base, r(75) = -.57, p < .001. Consistent with the group results, the dismissing rat- ing was also positively associated with the balance of control scales, K75) = .40, p < .001, and r(75) = .39, p < .001 for friend- ships and for romantic relationships, respectively.

The profile shown by the preoccupied group was opposite to that of the dismissing in almost every respect; the means of the two groups differed significantly on each of the 15 rating

230 KIM BARTHOLOMEW AND LEONARD M. HOROWITZ

Table 1 Mean Interview Ratings Across Attachment Groups

Model

Self Other

n

General Elaboration Coherence

Friendships Self-disclosure Intimacy Balance of control

Romantic relationships Highest level involvement Balance of control

Personal characteristics Self-confidence Emotional expressiveness Crying frequency

Interpersonal characteristics Warmth Reliance on others Others as secure base Nonsocial vs social crying Caregiving

Secure

Positive Positive

36

6.84, 6.08c

4.00b 5.09c 2.94C

3.6 le 3.29b

3.54C 3.76b 2.52b

3.13C 4.25b 3.34b 2.50b 4.25b

Dismissing

Positive Negative

14

Measure

5.74, 5.45b

3.33, 4.10, 3.10e

2.60B 3.73C

3.93d 2.60. 1.45.

2.21, 3.268 2.43a 2.17,, 3.79,

Preoccupied

Negative Positive

11

7.95C 4.76,

5.12C 4.54b 2.62fl

3.61C 2.91a,b

2.86b 5.2Oc 3.32C

2.94^ 5.I5C 4.I2C 3.70c 4.80c

Fearful

Negative Negative

16

6.23^ 5.00^b

3.05, 3-79, 2.608

2.60, 2.72a

2.24a 3.39b 2.17,,

2.70,, 3.46, 2.61. l.75b 4.25^,

I\% 76)

6.90* 16.54*

19.40* 23.12*

7.94*

6.06* 8.50*

27.77* 12.32* 5.75*

11.77* 14.65* 21.09*

7.60* 6.51*

Note, Means with different subscripts differ significantly at p < .05 according to Newman-Keuls pairwise comparisons. *p<.001.

scales. The preoccupied group scored uniquely high on ela- boration, self-disclosure (showing a tendency to disclose inap- propriately), emotional expressiveness, frequency of crying, reli- ance on others, use of others as a secure base, crying in the presence of others, and caregiving. They were also rated high on level of romantic involvement and low on coherence and bal- ance of control in friendships. Continuous ratings of the preoc- cupied prototype correlated positively with the presence of elab- oration, r(75) = .61, p < .001, emotional expressiveness, r(75) = .78, p < .001, level of romantic involvements, r(75) = .34, p < .01, disclosure, ii75) = .60, p < .001, the tendency to rely on others, r(75) = .64, p < .001, use others as a secure base, r(15) – .60, p < .001, and caregiving, r{75) = .38, /? < .001. The preoccu- pied rating was also positively correlated with the tendency to cry frequently, r{75) = .61, p < .001, and in the company of others, r(75) = .45, p < .001, and negatively correlated with the balance of control in friendships, r(75) = -.35, p < .01, coher- ence, r(75) = -.39, p < .001, and self-confidence, r{75) = – .33, p<.001.

Finally, the fearful group was rated significantly lower than the secure and preoccupied on self-disclosure, intimacy, level of romantic involvement, reliance on others, and use of others as a secure base when upset. They were also rated as uniquely low in self-confidence and as low on both balance-of-control scales. Continuous ratings of the fearful prototype showed negative correlations with self-confidence, r(75) – -.70, p < .001, and

coherence, r{75) ~ -.35, p < .01, and with all measures indicat- ing closeness of relationships—including self-disclosure, K75) = -.43, p < .001, intimacy, r(75) = -.52, p < .001, level of involvement in romantic relationships, r(75) = —.36, p < .01, capacity to rely on others, r(75) = -.30, p < .01, and use of others as a secure base, r(75) = -.33, p < .01. The fearful rating was also negatively correlated with the balance of control mea- sures for both friends, r(75) = —.40, p < .001, and romantic relationships, r(75) = —.32, p < .01, indicating a tendency to assume a subservient role in close relationships.

A discriminant analysis was performed to assess the degree to which the various interview ratings accounted for the overall discrimination between the four attachment groups. The analy- sis (using a stepwise method with minimization of Wilks’s lambda as the selection criterion) resulted in three significant discriminant functions, which correctly classified 92% of the sample—including 86% of the secure group, 94% of the fearful group, and 100% of both the preoccupied and dismissing groups. Twelve of the 15 interview rating scales independently contributed significant variance to the final functions, con- firming that the group distinctions are multiply determined.

Sex Differences in Attachment Ratings

Female subjects received significantly higher ratings than male subjects on the interview-based preoccupied rating

ATTACHMENT STYLES 231

(women’s M= 3.10, men’s M = 2.00), z(75) = 2.88, p < .01; male subjects received significantly higher ratings than female sub- jects on the interview-based dismissing rating (women’s M = 3.10, men’s M= 4.01), t(15) = 2.70, p < .01. To control for these sex differences, sex of subject was included as a covariate in all correlational and group analyses.

Depth of Friendships

On average, the pairs of friends reported reasonably close friendships of 1 to 2 years’ duration. The four groups did not differ significantly in the length of the friendships according to either subjects, F(3, 72) = 1.22, ns, or their accompanying friend, F(3, 72) = 0.75, ns. In addition, there were no group differences in the composite measure of closeness of friend- ships according to either subjects, F(3,72) = 1.82, ns, or friends, F(3, 72) = 0.37, ns. Therefore, the group differences reported below cannot be explained by a difference in either the length or the closeness of the friendships.

Intercorrelations of Attachment Styles

The pattern of correlations among the four attachment rat- ings for each method was hypothesized to be consistent with the model presented in Figure 1. Interview attachment ratings in opposing positions were negatively correlated: Between the secure and fearful ratings, r(75) = — .55, p< .001; between the preoccupied and dismissing ratings, r(15) = -.50, p < .001. The correlations between styles in adjacent positions showed non- significant or low negative correlations with one another (range = —.26 to —.14). Similar patterns were found for the self-report and friend-report attachment ratings (on the Rela- tionship Questionnaire): The secure and fearful ratings were negatively correlated (respective rs = -.65 and -.69, ps < .001), and the preoccupied and dismissing ratings were negatively correlated (respective rs = -.37 and —.41, ps < .001). For both self- and friend-reports, the styles in adjacent positions showed nonsignificant or negative correlations with one another with one exception: Friend ratings of the fearful and dismissing styles were positively correlated, r(J5) = .27, p < .05.

Multidimensional scalings were performed to determine if the dimensional structure underlying the intercorrelations of the attachment ratings were consistent with the proposed model. Intercorrelations among the four continuous attach- ment ratings were used as measures of proximity. A separate correlation matrix was prepared for each set of attachment rat- ings—interview ratings, the subjects’ own ratings of their match to each attachment style (on the Relationship Question- naire), and the friends’ ratings of subjects on each attachment style (on the friend version of the Relationship Questionnaire). Each matrix was then subjected to a nonmetric multidimen- sional analysis using the program KYST (Kruskal, Young, & Seery, 1973). The one-dimensional solutions yielded stress val- ues from .38 to .50 (Stress Formula 2; Kruskal & Wish, 1978), whereas the two-dimensional solutions yielded a stress value of .00 in each case. To facilitate comparisons with the theoretical model, the axes were rotated so that the secure placement was in the upper left quadrant at 45° from each axis. As indicated in

Figure 2, each set of data yielded a configuration roughly corre- sponding to the structure proposed in Figure 1.

In addition, a factor analysis was performed to examine the convergence of interview ratings, the subjects* self-ratings, and the friends’ ratings. Figure 3 shows the results of a principal- components analysis with varimax rotation of the intercorrela- tions of the three sets of attachment ratings (with axes rotated to facilitate interpretation). The two factors accounted for 47% of the variance.1

Self-Concept Measures

Subjects in the two groups that were theoretically expected to reflect a negative self-image were hypothesized to exhibit lower scores on measures of self-concept than subjects in the two groups that were theoretically expected to reflect a positive self- image. Using subject classifications derived from the interview- based attachment ratings, a 2 (positive vs. negative self-image) X 2 (positive vs. negative other-image) multivariate analysis of co- variance (MANCOVA; using sex as a covariate) was therefore performed on the three self-concept measures: self-esteem, self- acceptance, and subjective distress. The results showed a signifi- cant main effect for the self factor only, as hypothesized, FQ, 70) = 7.11, p < .001. Follow-up univariate analyses showed significant effects for the self factor for each of the three mea- sures (all ps < .001; see Table 2 for group means). No other main effect or interaction was significant.

Correlational analyses confirmed this pattern of results. Rat- ings of the degree to which subjects matched the secure and dismissing prototypes were positively correlated with measures of self-concept (partial correlations, controlling for sex, ranged from .20 to .41, all ps < .05), whereas ratings of the degree to which subjects matched the fearful and preoccupied prototypes were negatively correlated with measures of self-concept (par- tial correlations ranged from —.18 to -.49, all ps < .06).

Sociability Measure

On the other hand, the model implies that subjects in the two groups that were expected to reflect a positive image of others would exhibit higher scores on the measure of sociability than subjects in the two groups expected to reflect a negative image of others. Therefore, a 2 (positive vs. negative self-image) X 2 (positive vs. negative other-image) analysis of covariance (AN- COVA) was performed on the sociability measure (with sex as a covariate). The results showed a significant main effect for the other factor only, as hypothesized, F(l, 72)= 19.96,p<.001 (see Table 2 for group means). Correspondingly, sociability was posi- tively correlated (controlling for sex) with the secure and preoc- cupied prototype ratings, r(14) = .36, p < .001, and r{74) = .24, p < .05, and negatively correlated with the fearful and dismiss- ing prototype ratings, r{lA) = – .41, p < .001, and r(74) = -.20, p < .05.

1 Although the different sources of attachment ratings tended to con- verge, the correlations between specific attachment ratings across methods were not high. The average correlation between correspond- ing ratings across interview and self-reports was. 34; the average corre- lation across interview and friend-reports was .25.

232 KIM BARTHOLOMEW AND LEONARD M. HOROWITZ

Interview

\X>*

0 . 3 –

.1.0 •

Self-report Friend-report

1.0-

0 J –

•OS-

•in-

secure

A

A

Dismissing

Preoccupied

A

A

Fearful

1.0-

0.3-

0 –

-0.3-

-1.0 •

Secure

A

A Dismissing

L – ~ i 1 1 1

Preoccupied

A

A

Fearful ‘ ” 1 ” – ‘ 1 -T” ‘ 1 ”

• iJO -OS 0 0.3 1 4 ‘ * -0-3 0

Figure 2. Multidimensional scalings of interview, self-report, and friend-report attachment ratings.

0.5 1.0

ATTACHMENT STYLES 233

Secure ^

r Is

\ 1 1 -0.8

Dismissing

.-—”

-0.4

/ a /

-0.4

-0.8

0.8

0.4

1

>

L

/

0.4

>

Preoccupied

^ aj

i i i i i i

0.8

y/ Fearful

O Interview ratings

A Self-report ratings

• Friend-report ratings

Figure 3. Factor analysis of interv iew, self-report, and friend-report attachment ratings.

Self-report ratings of sociability were corroborated by friend- reports. The main effect for positivity of other models was repli- cated, F(\, 72) = 7.08, p < .001, but there was also a significant effect for the self model factor, F{\, 72) = 3.12, p < .05. Inspec- tion of means (see Table 2) indicated that friends tended to see the dismissing group as especially low on sociability: Follow-up Newman-Keuls comparisons indicated that the mean of the dismissing group was significantly lower than that of each of the other attachment groups.

Interpersonal Problems

Finally, we examined the location within the two-dimen- sional interpersonal space of subjects’ interpersonal problems, as reported on the UP. First, each subject’s mean response across all problems was examined. A two-way ANCOVA re- vealed a significant main effect for the self factor only, F(l, 72) = 11.82, p < .001. The two groups with a negative self-image re- ported higher levels of interpersonal problems overall than the groups with a positive self-image. Parallel analyses with friends’ reports revealed a trend for groups with negative self models to be judged to experience higher mean levels of interpersonal problems, F(l, 71) = 2.93, p < .10. Therefore, in the following analyses, each subject’s score on each of the eight subscales (for both self- and friend-reports) was converted to an “ipsative score.” That is, each subject’s subscale scores were expressed as a

deviation from that subject’s overall mean, thereby reflecting the extent to which that group of problems was more or less problematic for that particular person.2 Each subscale score was then expressed as a z score using the mean and standard deviation of that subscale for the entire sample.

Each of the eight subscale scores was correlated with each prototype rating, and the mean of each subscale was also com- puted for each prototype group. Group means on the circum- plex subscales, for self- and friend-reports, are shown graphi- cally in Figure 4. Significant group differences were found on six of the eight subscales for both self-reports and friend-re- ports. Correlational analyses for self-reports and friend-reports are contained in Table 3. All of the self-report and friend-report problem subscales were significantly correlated with at least one of the attachment ratings.

As shown in Figure 4, the secure group’s profile of means was elevated on the warm side of the interpersonal space according to both self- and friend-reports. However, no one subscale score was extreme: The largest standardized mean scores were on the order of 0.25. Correlations between secure ratings and the self- report circumplex subscales revealed significant, although mod- est, positive correlations with the overly expressive and autocra- tic scales, as well as negative correlations with the cold and introverted scales. The friends’ reports showed positive correla- tions with the exploitable and nurturant scales and negative correlations with the cold and introverted scales.

The dismissing group showed a self-reported profile centered on the hostile side of the interpersonal space. Correlational analyses indicated that the cold subscale is most highly corre- lated with this attachment style. The friends generally con- firmed this pattern, although friends1 reports tended to show the dismissing style as more strongly associated with introver- sion than self-reports did. Also, the dismissing rating was nega- tively correlated with both self- and friend-ratings of the nurtur- ant and expressive scales and with self-ratings of the subasser- tive and exploitable scales.

The self-report profile of the preoccupied group showed an elevation on the overly expressive scale, with the correlational analyses confirming this location of interpersonal problems. Friends’ reports also described these subjects as highest on the

2 When a personality inventory has many subscales, it is sometimes necessary to ipsatize subjects’ subscale scores. This procedure has been shown to be particularly important for the IIP because a principal- components analysis of the IIP items typically yields a large general first factor that reflects differences among people in their readiness to endorse complaints (Horowitz, Rosenberg, Baer, Ureno, & Villasenor, 1988). That factor may be considered a complaint factor, an acquies- cence factor, or an intensity factor, and as shown by Wiggins, Steiger, and Gaelick (1981, p. 283), it reflects individual differences in the use of the response format, rather than differences in important aspects of interpersonal functioning. Therefore, the person’s average response level must be treated separately from the circumplex components per se. The preferred way to control for individual differences in people’s average response level is to ipsatize the subscale scores by expressing each score as a deviation from the subject’s own mean across all sub- scales (Cronbach, 1949; Horowitz et al., 1988; Strack, 1987; Wiggins & Pincus, 1989). The ipsatizing procedure has been shown to improve the circumplex properties of interpersonal measures (Paddock & No- wicki, 1986; Rinn, 1965; Wiggins et al., 1981).

234 KIM BARTHOLOMEW AND LEONARD M. HOROWITZ

Table 2 Means of Self-Report Self-Concept Measures and Self and Friend-Report Sociability Across Attachment Groups

Model

Self Other

n

Self-concept Distress Self-esteem Self-acceptance

Sociability Self-report Friend-report

Secure

Positive Positive

36

3.34 35.28

3.75

3.62 3.48

Dismissing

Positive Negative

14

Measure

3.00 36.50

3.87

3.07 2.63

Preoccupied

Negative Positive

11

4.30 32.21

3.39

3.87 3.71

Fearful

Negative Negative

16

4.19 30.69

3.19

2.91 3.31

Note. Higher numbers reflect higher distress, self-esteem, self-acceptance, and sociability.

overly expressive scale, but the friends’ means were also ele- vated in other octants reflecting dominance (i.e.; the autocratic and competitive scales). Correspondingly, the preoccupied rat- ing was positively associated with friend-ratings of the expres- sive, autocratic, and competitive subscales. That is, the friends’ descriptions of the subjects’ problems revealed less overall warmth and more overall dominance than the subjects1 self-re- ports revealed. Preoccupied ratings were also negatively corre- lated with self-ratings on the cold scale and friend-ratings on the introverted and exploitable scales.

Finally, the fearful group reported relatively more problems reflecting a lack of assertiveness and social inhibition (introver- sion). The corresponding friends’ reports confirmed this gen- eral pattern of results, as did the correlational analyses for both sets of data. Also, the fearful rating was negatively correlated with both self- and friend-ratings of the autocratic scale and with self-ratings of the competitive and expressive scales.

Conclusions

This study demonstrated that a semi-structured interview can be used to assess the degree to which subjects approximate each of the four hypothesized attachment styles. An additional 15 interview rating scales demonstrated the distinctiveness of each of the four styles. The internal structures of the interview ratings, the self-report ratings, and the friend-report ratings of the four attachment styles were consistent with the proposed model. Thus, a two-dimensional structure spatially reproduced the hypothesized relationships among the four styles, and the three sets of ratings independently produced similar results. The data also supported the hypothesis that the two groups theoretically described as having a positive self-model would differ on measures of self-concept from the two groups theoreti- cally described as having a negative self-model. Also as hypoth- esized, the two groups theoretically described as having a posi- tive model of others differed on a measure of sociability from the two groups described as having a negative model of others.

Taken together, the results supported the implication of Bowlby’s theory that four different attachment styles can be identified.

The circumplex analyses confirmed the hypothesis that each style is associated with a distinct pattern of interpersonal prob- lems. Fearful subjects were most likely to report interpersonal problems in the overly passive region of the interpersonal space (the lower quadrants), whereas dismissing subjects were more likely to report problems related to a lack of warmth in social interactions. The one group whose problems did not conform to the original hypothesis was the preoccupied group. Whereas their negative self-image and positive other-image were ex- pected to reflect problems in being overly warm and passive (the lower right quadrant), their problems reflected a greater degree of warmth-dominance (the upper right quadrant). Ex- amples of items falling in this region of the interpersonal space are “I want to be noticed too much” and “it is hard for me to stay out of other people’s business.” These findings suggest that al- though preoccupied people are highly dependent on others to maintain positive self-regard, they attempt to achieve this aim though a controlling (overly dominating) interpersonal style. Finally, the data also showed that self- and friend-reports of interpersonal problems were generally consistent across the four attachment styles.

Study 2

Study 1 examined ratings of the attachment styles that were based on the subjects’ descriptions of their close friendships and romantic relationships, and these assessments were shown to predict important aspects of subjects’ personalities and inter- personal functioning. Study 2 was designed (a) to replicate the circumplex analyses of Study I, (b) to extend the proposed model of attachment to representations of relationships within the family of origin, and (c) to investigate the relationships be- tween family and peer attachment representations. To examine these questions, the subjects of Study 2 were interviewed about

ATTACHMENT STYLES 235 Autocratic Autocratic

Competitive

Introverted

Subassertive

Expressive

Exploitable

Competitive

Cold

Introverted

Expressive

Nurturant

Exploitable

Subassertive

Competitive

Introverted

DISMISSING

Autocratic

Expressive

Nurturant

Exploitable

Competitive

Introverted

FEARFUL

Autocratic

Expressive

Nurturant

Exploitable

Subassertive Subassertive

Self-report Friend-report

Figure 4. Self-report and friend-report profiles of interpersonal problems across attachment groups.

both family relationships and peer relationships, and each in- terview was used independently to rate the person according to their approximation to each of the four attachment prototypes.

It was expected that both sets of attachment ratings would reproduce spatially, the structure hypothesized by the model. Attachment theory suggests that people’s representations of fa-

milial relationships predispose them toward particular styles of relating to friends; therefore, independent assessments across the two domains were expected to be positively correlated. De- spite this correlation, however, family and peer attachment as- sessments may contribute in different ways to a person’s current interpersonal difficulties, so this study also examined the rela-

236 KIM BARTHOLOMEW AND LEONARD M. HOROWITZ

Table 3 Correlations of Continuous Attachment Ratings and Self- and Friend-Report Interpersonal Problem Scales

Scale

Autocratic Competitive Cold Introverted Subassertive Exploitable Nurturant Expressive

Secure

Self

.22*

.05 -.27** -.37*** -.11

.12

.19

.20*

Friend

.03 -.04 -.29** -.23* -.01

.23*

.22*

.12

Dismissing

Self

.03

.16

.58***

.10 -.20* -.22* -.24* -.20*

Friend

-.02 .00 .46*** .32**

-.09 -.18 -.27** -.24*

Preoccupied

Self

.15

.14 -.35*** -.15 -.12 -.11

.04

.41***

Friend

.33**

.24* -.16 -.32** -.18 – .21*

.04

.32**

Fearful

Self

-.35*** -.22*

.03

.45***

.38***

.16 -.06 -.43***

Friend

-.27** -.14

.04

.25*

.20**

.06 -.08 -.14

Note. N = 77. Correlations are partial (controlling for sex) and are based on ipsative scorings of circum- plex scales. * p < .05. ** p < .01. ***p< .001.

tive power of the two sets of ratings to predict people’s interper- sonal problems.

Method

Subjects

Subjects were 33 female and 36 male students in introductory psy- chology classes who agreed to participate in an “interview about social relations” for course credit. They ranged in age from 17 to 24 (M = 19.5), and their ethnic composition was 79% White, 9% Hispanic, 6% Asian, 3% Black, and 3% other.

Procedure

Subjects were tested in sessions that were 1 to 2 weeks apart; each session lasted approximately 1 hr. In the first session, subjects were randomly assigned to either a male interviewer (an advanced under- graduate psychology major) or a female interviewer (the first author) and were interviewed about their relationships with family and then with friends. In the second session, subjects completed a self-report packet of questionnaires.

Attachment interview. The first half of the interview focused on the subject’s representations of family relationships. Subjects were asked about their family backgrounds, the quality of their relationships with each parent from their earliest memories to the present, their reactions to being separated from parents, their feelings of being rejected by parents, and parental reactions to their emotional upsets as a child. Subjects were also asked to make general evaluations of their child- hood experiences in the family and to describe how these experiences had influenced them. The second half of the interview was an abbre- viated version of the interview described in Study 1. Each interview was tape-recorded.

Attachment ratings were based on prototypes of each of the four attachment styles. The peer attachment prototypes were identical to those used in Study 1. Comparable descriptions were prepared for styles of relating to family members (available from the first author). Four independent raters (two for the family section and two for the peer section) rated each interview. The raters were advanced undergraduate psychology majors or graduate students, three were female and one was male, and all were blind to other measures in the study. Raters listened to the relevant section of the tape-recorded interview and rated each subject on 7-point scales describing the degree to which that subject

matched each of the four attachment prototypes. The final ratings of a subject consisted of the mean of the two ratings for each style. Each subject thus received two sets of final ratings, one for the family attach- ment interview (hereafter called the family ratings) and one for the peer attachment interview (hereafter called the peer ratings). The reliabil- ities of the family ratings ranged from .75 to .86, and those of the peer ratings ranged from .74 to .88. As in Study 1, the highest of the four peer ratings was used to assign subjects to one of the four adult attachment patterns. This procedure resulted in the following distribution of sub- jects across the attachment groups: 57% secure (n= 39), 18% dismissing {n = 12), 10% preoccupied in = 7), and 15% fearful (n = 10).

Self-report measures. During the second session, each subject com- pleted the UP as well as a battery of other measures (e.g., parenting scales, self-description tasks) that are not relevant to this report. Coeffi- cient alphas for the subscales of the IIP ranged from .72 to .85.

Results

Sex Differences in Attachment Ratings

There was a significant sex difference on the preoccupied rating across both segments of the interview. For the family interview, the mean preoccupied’rating was 3.09 for women and 1.96 for men, t(61) = 3.10, p < .01; for the peer attachment interview, the mean preoccupied rating was 2.76 for women and 1.69 for men, t{61) = 2.72, p < .01. No sex differences occurred on any of the other ratings. Sex was entered as a covariate in all subsequent group and correlational analyses.

Intercorrelations of Attachment Ratings

For each set of correlations, the pattern was consistent with the proposed model. In both cases, attachment ratings that are diagonally opposed in the model showed significant negative correlations. For the family ratings, the correlation between the secure and fearful ratings was -.65 (p < .001 )T and that between the preoccupied and dismissing ratings was -.54 (p < .001). For the peer ratings, the correlation between the secure and fearful ratings was -.39 (p < .01), and that between the preoccupied and dismissing ratings was -.47 (p < .001). In both cases, rat- ings in adjacent positions showed uniformly low or 0 correla- tions, with rs ranging from -.32 to .06.

ATTACHMENT STYLES

A multidimensional scaling was performed on each set of intercorrelations among the continuous attachment ratings. For both the family and peer ratings, a two-dimensional solution with a stress value of zero was obtained (Stress Formula 2; one- dimensional solutions had stress values of .55 and .50, respec- tively). In each case, the resulting configuration was consistent with the proposed model (see Figure 5).

Family and Peer Attachment Ratings

As expected, corresponding family ratings and peer ratings were significantly correlated with one another. For the secure ratings, r(61) = .39, p < .001; for the fearful ratings, r(67) = .29, p < .01; for the preoccupied ratings, r(67) = .66, p < .001; and for the dismissing ratings, r(67) = .41, p < .001. In contrast, all correlations between noncorresponding ratings were nonsigni- ficant or negative (with a range from -.40 to .01).

Interpersonal Problems

A two-way ANCOVA (positive vs. negative self-image X posi- tive vs. negative other-image, controlling for sex) again revealed a significant self-model effect only for mean endorsement of interpersonal problems, F{\, 63) = 8.28, p< .01. The preoccu- pied and fearful groups expressed higher mean levels of inter- personal distress than the other two groups. Therefore, each circumplex subscale score was ipsatized by expressing it as a deviation from the subject’s overall mean level of interpersonal problems. Each subscale score was then standardized across the entire sample. Figure 6 presents the circumplex profiles for each attachment group. Significant group differences were ob- tained in a two-way ANCOVA on seven of the eight subscales. In addition, each insecure prototype rating was significantly correlated with five or six of the circumplex scales (see Table 4).

The secure attachment rating was not strongly related to the circumplex ratings: The secure group’s standardized subscale means were consistently close to zero. Moreover, the secure rat- ing only showed low correlations with two scales (positive with the expressive scale and negative with the introverted scale). The dismissing group showed its highest means on subscales reflecting excessive coldness, with the correlational analyses showing positive correlations with the cold and competitive subscales and negative correlations with the exploitable, nurtur- ant, and expressive scales. The preoccupied group showed ele- vated means on the subscales in the warm-dominant quadrant, and the continuous preoccupied rating was most strongly asso- ciated with the overly expressive subscale. The preoccupied tax- ing was also positively correlated with the nurturant and auto- cratic scales and negatively correlated with the cold, intro- verted, and subassertive scales. Finally, the fearful group showed elevated means on those subscales located in the pas- sive octants; the introverted subscale showed the highest mean. Correlational analyses also indicated that the fearful style was positively associated with problems of introversion, subasserti- veness, and the tendency to be exploited and negatively corre- lated with problems related to being overly nurturant, expres- sive, autocratic, and competitive.

1.0-

0.5-

Peer

1.0-

0.5-

-1.0 –

Secure

A

A Dismissing

Preoccupied

A

A

Fearful

-1.0 -0.5 0.5 1.0

Figure 5. Multidimensional scalings of family and peer interview attachment ratings.

Comparison of Study 1 and Study 2 Circumplex Analyses

To facilitate comparisons between the circumplex results of Studies 1 and 2, each subject’s eight subscale scores were used to

238 KIM BARTHOLOMEW AND LEONARD M. HOROWITZ

SECURE PREOCCUPIED

Autocratic Autocratic

Competitive

Introverted

Expressive

Nurturant

Exploitable

Competitive

Introverted

Expressive

urturant

Exploitable

Subassertlve Sub assertive

Competitive

Introverted

DISMISSING

Autocratic

Expressive

Nurturant

Exploitable

Competitive

Introverted

FEARFUL

Autocratic

Expressive

Nurturant

Exploitable

Subassertlve Subassertlve

Figure 6. Self-report profiles of interpersonal problems across attachment groups.

ATTACHMENT STYLES 239

Table 4 Correlations of Continuous Peer Attachment Ratings and Self- Report Interpersonal Problem Scales

Scale

Autocratic Competitive Cold Introverted Subassertive Exploitable Nurturant Expressive

Secure

,05 -.18 -.07 -.28* -.04 -.07

.14

.23*

Dismissing

.11

.29**

.43***

.12 -.04 -.25* -.24* -.33**

Preoccupied

.36**

.18 -.32** -.32** -.33**

.02

.26*

.42***

Fearful

-.51*** – . 2 9 ”

.02

.49***

.34**

.21* -.32** -.33**

Note. N = 69. Correlations are partial (controlling for sex) and are based on ipsative scorings of circumplex scales. *p<.05. **/><.0i. •••/>< .001.

calculate a mean rating along the nurturance and dominance dimensions in accordance with the procedure described by Wiggins, Phillips, and Trapnell (1989). Significant group differ- ences were obtained on the dimensional scores across both methods and studies according to one-way ANCOVAs (with sex as a covariate). Results for warmth and dominance scores, re- spectively, were Study 1 self-reports, F(3, 71) = 6.72, p < .001, and F(3, 71) = 5.45, p < .01; Study 1 friend-reports, F(3, 71) = 5.23, p < .01, and FQ, 71) = 2.71, p = .05; Study 2 self-reports, F(3, 63) = 4.52, p < .01, and F(3, 63) = 6.02, p < .001. The placement of the group centroids in the interpersonal space (that is, the mean warmth and dominance coordinates) from the self-reports and friend-reports of Study 1 and the self-re- ports of Study 2 are shown in Figure 7.

The three corresponding centroids for the secure group were in relatively close proximity to one another; in all three sets of data, the secure interpersonal style was associated with a less extreme interpersonal profile (that is, a mean placement closer to the origin) than the other three styles. The three centroids of the dismissing group were also close to one another, in all cases reflecting the salience of the overly cold subscale. The centroids of the preoccupied group showed the greatest diversity across the three sets (perhaps partly because of this group’s small size), but in all cases the centroid fell in the warm-dominant quad- rant, reflecting the salience of problems with overexpressive- ness in all three data sets. Finally, the angular locations of the centroids of the fearful group were also similar across the three assessments. In all three cases, the centroids showed the influ- ence of the introverted and subassertive scales.

Family and Peer Attachment Ratings as Predictors of Interpersonal Problems

The variables of warmth and dominance were used as sum- mary measures of the subject’s interpersonal problems, and a regression analysis was performed to predict these measures from the family ratings and from the peer ratings. In order to compare the predictive power of the family and peer ratings, a hierarchical regression analysis was performed twice. In both

analyses, sex was entered first in the equation. Then, in one case, the family ratings were entered as a block followed by the peer ratings; in the other case, the peer ratings were entered first.

First consider the prediction of the dominance aspect of in- terpersonal problems. When entered as the first variable in the equation (after sex), the family ratings accounted for 18% of the variance, F(4, 62) = 3.31, p < .05, and the peer ratings ac- counted for an additional 27%, F(4, 58) = 7.30, p < .001; but when the peer ratings were entered first, they accounted for 41% of the variance, F(4,62) = 10.71, p < .001, and the family ratings did not add significantly, F(4,58) = 1.15, ns. A different pattern was obtained in predicting the warmth aspect of inter- personal problems. In that case, when family ratings were en- tered first, they accounted for 31% of the variance, F(4, 62) = 7.50, p < .001, and the peer ratings added an additional 10%, F(4, 58) = 2.59, p < .05; when the order of entry was reversed, the peer ratings accounted for 28% of the variance, F(4, 62) = 6.53, p < .001, and the family ratings added an additional 13%, F(4, 59) = 3.34, p< .05. Thus, only peer ratings independently accounted for variation among people in the dominance aspect of interpersonal problems, whereas both sets of ratings contrib- uted independently to variation among people in the affiliative aspect of their problems.

Conclusion

The intercorrelations among the family ratings were consis- tent with the proposed model. In addition, family attachment

D Study 1 – Self-report O Study 1 – Friend-report A Study 2 – Self-report

Figure 7. Attachment group centroids of interpersonal problems for Study 1 self-reports and friend-reports and Study 2 self-reports.

240 KIM BARTHOLOMEW AND LEONARD M. HOROWITZ

ratings were moderately correlated with corresponding peer at- tachment ratings, suggesting that the four adult attachment styles are meaningfully related to, although by no means reduci- ble to, representations of childhood experiences. Thus, the pro- posed model of adult attachment is also applicable to represen- tations of family relations. In addition, the circumplex analyses largely replicated the findings of Study 1.

Finally, the results demonstrated that both the family and peer attachment ratings contributed to the prediction of the warmth and dominance dimensions underlying interpersonal problems. The peer ratings were strongly related to both di- mensions of interpersonal problems. In contrast, the family ratings only independently contributed to the prediction of problems along the warmth dimension.

According to the circumplex model of interpersonal behav- ior, behaviors on the affiliation dimension tend to invite corre- sponding responses (e.ĝ warmth invites warmth) and, there- fore, tend to be self-perpetuating. Thus, in a longitudinal study of child rearing, Schaefer and Bayley (1963) found considerably higher consistency over time for the warmth dimension of ma- ternal behavior than for the dominance (control) dimension. The degree of warmth experienced and expressed within the family of origin may therefore be particularly salient in family attachment representations, which may in turn mediate the af- filiation dimensions of peer relations, at least up to late adoles- cence.

In contrast, determinants of dominance may be more com- plex and subject to greater variation over time and across rela- tionships. Schaefer and Bayley (1963) suggest that because chil- dren’s needs for control and autonomy change radically over the course of development, parents may tend to shift to more or less age-appropriate behaviors on this dimension over time. Thus, to the extent that dominance imbalances exist in child-parent relationships, they may be less stable, hence a less salient char- acteristic of family attachment representations, than the affec- tive tone of the relationships. Furthermore, in relationships of dominance, two sides of the relationship may be learned: A person who is typically dominated learns both to submit and to dominate. For instance, Troy and Sroufe (1987) present evi- dence that avoidant preschoolers have internalized both the victim and victimizing aspects of their relationship with par- ents and may be capable of assuming either role depending on their social partner. Therefore, problems with dominance may be best understood in terms of the person’s history with peers.

General Discussion

This article examined a model of individual differences in adult attachment in which two underlying dimensions, the per- son’s internal model of the self (positive or negative) and the person’s internal model of others (positive or negative), were used to define four attachment patterns. A prototypic descrip- tion of each style was generated, and each subject’s degree of correspondence to each prototype was assessed through a semi- structured interview concerning the person’s current relation- ships with peers and early relationships within the family. Multi- dimensional scalings confirmed the hypothesized underlying structure. Across family ratings and peer ratings, and across interview, self-reports, and friend-reports, configurations of at-

tachment ratings conformed to the theoretical model. The data also showed a convergence between family and peer ratings and between ratings from the interview, self-reports, and friend’s reports. Self-concept measures differentiated the attachment styles with respect to the model of self only, and measures of sociability differentiated the styles with respect to the model of other only. Differences in interpersonal problems were also ex- amined, and circumplex analyses indicated that each attach- ment style was associated with a distinct profile of interper- sonal problems. The findings were, for the most part, consis- tent for self-reports and friend-reports and for self-reports across the two studies.

The results of this research confirm that the valence of both self-models and models of others are separate, important di- mensions of an adult’s orientation to close relationships and that the two dimensions can vary independently. The present findings thus underscore one limitation of conceptualizing dif- ficulties with intimacy as simply either overdependency or avoidance of intimacy. In the present studies, the two groups with a negative model of the self (the preoccupied and fearful) showed similar responses to measures of personal insecurity, but they differed on measures indicating readiness to become intimate with and rely on other people. The two groups also showed diametrically opposite patterns of interpersonal prob- lems: Whereas the problems of the preoccupied subjects were centered in the warm-dominant quadrant, those of the fearful subjects were centered in the cold-passive quadrant.

Similarly, the two groups defined as avoidant of close rela- tionships (the fearful and dismissing) both showed difficulties in becoming close to and relying on others, but they differed significantly on measures reflecting an internalized sense of self-worth. Only the fearful style was consistently associated with social insecurity and lack of assertiveness. Thus, the com- mon assumption that people who maintain interpersonal dis- tance are driven by a fear of intimacy would seem to be an oversimplification. In both cases, however, the person’s avoid- ance of intimacy preempts the possibility of establishing close relationships that might otherwise help the person update work- ing models of other people.

This project raises a number of important theoretical issues. For one, research has consistently demonstrated a positive rela- tion between self-acceptance and acceptance of others (e.g., Fey, 1955; Phillips, 1951). In attachment terms, models of the self and models of other people are postulated to have a common origin in early interpersonal interactions and are therefore gen- erally expected to be “complementary and mutually confirm- ing” (Bowlby, 1973, p. 204). In fact, the majority of subjects in these studies were judged to display attachment styles with con- gruent self- and other-models. However, two of the four attach- ment styles, the preoccupied and dismissing styles, were defined in terms of differing valences of self- and other-models. The present research has thus identified interpersonal patterns with discrepant valences between self- and other-acceptance (cf. Ep- stein & Feist, 1988). These two groups demonstrate two strate- gies that people may use at times to cope with unwelcome social information. In the preoccupied’style, people blame themselves for perceived rejections by others and are thereby able to main- tain a positive view of others. In the dismissing style, people downplay the importance of others whom they have experi-

ATTACHMENT STYLES 241

enced as rejecting and are thereby able to maintain high self-es- teem. Thus, models of the self and of others can complement and mutually confirm one another without being congruent.

None of the subjects in this project uniquely fit any one at- tachment prototype. Instead, most subjects reported a mix of tendencies across time and within and across relationships: Many subjects were rated as showing elements of two, three, and occasionally all four of the attachment styles. Therefore, a great deal of individual variability was lost when the four continuous ratings were collapsed into a simple, four-category classifica- tion. Nonetheless, the two approaches produced almost identi- cal patterns of results across dependent measures. That is, the conclusions were very similar whether a correlational analysis was used (involving four continuous ratings) or whether a be- tween-groups comparison of four groups was used. Thus, the present studies did not provide any convincing evidence for the advantage of continuous attachment ratings over a group classi- fication. However, the continuous ratings may have advantages that will emerge in future research. For one, continuous ratings allow an investigator to more precisely assess individual differ- ences. For example, two persons classified the same way may differ in the intensity of their highest ratings or in the pattern- ing of their secondary ratings, and those differences may be significant (e.g., in understanding clinical phenomena). Corre- lational analyses may also be particularly useful for samples that are not sufficiently large for group analyses or in which one or more attachment groups is underrepresented. Therefore, ad- ditional studies of diverse populations need to compare results obtained with each method before we can be clear about their relative advantages. Future work should consider also the rela- tive merits of measuring attachment patterns in terms of two dimensions rather than four prototypes. In fact, both of these issues are currently being explored with the aid of structural equation modeling (Griffin & Bartholomew, 1991).

The individual differences explored in this project fall within the normal range of attachment patterns for a student popula- tion. Although a few subjects reported that their social difficul- ties were a source of serious depression or anxiety, most subjects in these studies were reasonably well functioning. For instance, all of the subjects in Study 1 had a friend who agreed to accom- pany them in the study. Also, although some subjects described difficult family backgrounds, the majority came from intact upper-middle-class families. The fact that significant group dif- ferences emerged despite such a restricted range reflects the robustness of the reported relationships. Nonetheless, it is im- portant for future research to extend the procedure to other age groups and other populations. One important direction would be to examine clinical populations and determine whether the attachment styles are distinctively related to specific forms of psychopathology and whether certain attachment styles make a person more or less amenable to specific forms of treatment. People with a dismissing attachment style, for example, might be so much less invested in other people that they would be less amenable to forms of treatment that required an exploration of interpersonal interactions.

The model proposed in this article is the first to provide a theoretical rationale as to why four, rather than three, distinct attachment patterns are expected and the first to specify the expected relations among attachment styles. It is also the first

work to assess attachment representations of both familial and peer relationships and to use multiple assessment methods (in- terviews, self-reports, and friend-reports). However, several studies have reported results consistent with the proposed model. Collins and Read (1990) have reported that one possible solution to a cluster analysis of three continuous measures of adult attachment was four clusters in which two anxious styles were differentiated. Brennan, Shaver, and Tobey (in press) have recently compared the four-category model (as measured by the self-report Relationship Questionnaire) and the traditional three-category model (using the 1987 Hazan and Shaver mea- sure). They found that fearful subjects tended to endorse both the avoidant and ambivalent options on the Hazan measure and that a two-dimensional structure underlay the attachment rat- ings as hypothesized by the four-category model. In the child- hood attachment literature, a fourth style characterized by a mix of ambivalence and avoidance and associated with parental abuse and neglect has recently been reported by Main and Solo- mon (1990; the disorganized or D pattern) and by Crittenden (1988; the avoidant/ambivalent or A/C pattern). Moreover, the possibility of a relation between the fearful style and the D or A/C types is suggested by Latty-Manris finding that adult chil- dren of problem drinkers tended to endorse both the ambiva- lent and avoidant options on the Hazan measure (Latty-Mann & Davis, 1988) and Brennan’s finding that adult children of problem drinkers were overrepresented among those reporting a fearful style (Brennan et al., in press). However, future studies are clearly required to determine the precise relations between the proposed four-category model and previous conceptualiza- tions of individual differences in attachment.

Another important next step is to identify the mechanisms by which an attachment style is maintained. In processing so- cial information, people seem to produce behaviors that evoke specific reactions from other people, and this social feedback is interpreted in ways that confirm the person’s internal models of the self and others (see Caspi & Elder, 1988; Swann, 1983,1987). In fact, it is precisely “because persons select and create later social environments that early relationships are viewed as hav- ing special importance” (Sroufe & Fleeson, 1986, p. 68). For instance, selective affiliation in the form of the seeking or avoid- ance of social contacts and the selection of social partners who are likely to confirm internal models is expected to be central in maintaining adult patterns of attachment (see Collins & Read, 1990; Davis & Kirkpatrick, 1991). A related process, which Caspi and Elder (1988) call “interactional continuity” (p. 232), involves structuring social interactions so as to induce social partners to engage in self-confirming interaction patterns. Al- though the circumplex placements of the various attachment groups are suggestive of their characteristic interaction strate- gies, naturalistic observation is still needed to describe their actual social behavior and the reactions that that behavior tends to evoke in social partners. Finally, a whole array of informa- tion-processing biases are available to guide the processing of social feedback so as to confirm internal models (see Green- wald, 1982; Swann, 1983). Internal models are expected to di- rect attention, organize and filter new information, and deter- mine the accessibility of past experiences. Thereby, ambiguous stimuli (which may form the bulk of all social stimuli) tend to be assimilated to existing models.

242 KIM BARTHOLOMEW AND LEONARD M. HOROWITZ

Given the multiple pathways through which representations of attachment relations may perpetuate self-confirming social experiences, perhaps the more difficult question is how such representations come to be modified. Epstein (1980) argues that compelling emotional experiences that are inconsistent with existing models are required to change them. Such experi- ences are likely to arise within emotionally significant relation- ships, such as those with a spouse or therapist. Research has documented the potential therapeutic value of supportive spou- sal relationships in moderating the effects of difficult early at- tachment relationships (e.g., Brown & Harris, 1978; Crocken- berg, 1987; Quinton, Rutter, & Liddle, 1984). Major life transi- tions involving the adoption of new social roles (such as leaving for college, getting married, having children, or retiring) may also be opportune times for evaluating and potentially reorgan- izing attachment representations (cf. Caspi & Elder, 1988; Ricks, 1985). It has also been suggested that by coming to un- derstand and forgive parents for their parenting weaknesses or by achieving an autonomous stance in relation to parents, peo- ple can overcome the influence of early models (Main et al., 1985; Ricks, 1985). Thus, a challenge for future research is to explore empirically how attachment patterns are externalized, maintained, and revised in interaction with the social environ- ment.

References

Ainsworth, M. D. S. (1982). Attachment: Retrospect and prospect. In C. M. Parkes & J. Stevenson-Hi nde (Eds.), The place of attachment in human behavior, (pp. 3-30). New York: Basic Books.

Ainsworth, M. D. S. (1989). Attachments beyond infancy. American Psychologist, 44, 709-716.

Ainsworth, M. D. S., Blehar, M. G, Waters, E., & Wall, S. (1978). Pat- terns of attachment: A psychological study of the strange situation. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Alden, L. E., Wiggins, J. S., & Pincus, A. L. (1990). Construction of circumplex scales for the Inventory of Interpersonal Problems. Jour- nal of Personality Assessment, 55, 521-536.

Bartholomew, K.. (1990). Avoidance of intimacy: An attachment per- spective. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 7, 147-178.

Belsky, 1, & Pensky, E. (1988). Developmental history, personality, and family relationships: Toward an emergent family system. In R. A. Hinde & J. Stevenson-Hi nde (Eds.), Relationships within families (pp. 193-217). Oxford, England: Clarendon Press.

Bowlby, J. (1973). Attachment and loss: Vol. 2. Separation. New York: Basic Books.

Bowlby, J. (1977). The making and breaking of aflectional bonds. Brit- ish Journal of Psychiatry, 130, 201-210.

Bowlby, J. (1980). Attachment and loss: Vol. 3. Loss, sadness and depres- sion. New York: Basic Books.

Bowlby, J. (1982a). Attachment and loss: Retrospect and prospect. American Journal ofOrthopsychiatry, 52, 664-678.

Bowlby, J. (1982b). Attachment and loss: Vol. J. Attachment (2nd ed.). New “Vbrk: Basic Books. (Original work published 1969).

Brennan, K. A., Shaver, P. R., & Tobey, A. E. (in press). Attachment styles, gender, and parental problem drinking. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships.

Bretherton, I. (1985). Attachment theory: Retrospect and prospect. In I. Bretherton & E. Waters (Eds.), Growing points in attachment theory and research. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 50(1-2, Serial No. 209), 3-35.

Brown, G. W, & Harris, T. (1978). Social origins of depression. London: Tavistock.

Cantor, N., Smith, E. E., French, R. deS., & Mezzich, J. (1980). Psychiat- ric diagnosis as prototype categorization. Journal of Abnormal Psy- chology, 89,181-193.

Caspi, A., & Elder, G. H. (1988). Emergent family patterns: The inter- generational construction of problem behavior and relationships. In R. A. Hinde & J. Steven son-Hi nde (Eds.), Relationships within fami- lies (pp. 218-240). Oxford, England: Clarendon Press.

Cassidy, J. (1988). Child-mother attachment and the self in six-year- olds. Child Development, 59,121-134.

Cheek, J. M., &Buss, A. H. (1981). Shyness and sociability. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 41, 330-339.

Collins, N. L., &Read, S. J. (1990). Adult attachment, working models, and relationship quality in dating couples. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 644-663.

Crittenden, P. M. (1988). Relationships at risk. In J. Belsky & T. Nez- worski (Eds.), Clinical implications of attachment, (pp. 136-174). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Crockenberg, S. (1987). Predictors and correlates of anger toward and punitive control of toddlers by adolescent mothers. Child Develop- ment, 55,964-975.

Cronbach, L. J. (1949). Statistical methods applied to Rorschach scores: A review. Psychological Bulletin, 46, 393-429.

Crowell, J. A., & Feldman, S. S. (1987). Mothers’ internal models of relationships and children’s behavioral and developmental status: A study of mother-child interaction. Child Development, 59, 1273- 1285.

Davis, K. E., & Kirkpatrick, L. A. (1991). Attachment style, gender, and relationship stability: A longitudinal analysis. Unpublished manu- script, Department of Psychology, University of South Carolina,

Epstein, S. (1980). Self-concept: A review and the proposal of an inte- grated theory of personality. In E. Staub (Ed.), Personality: Basic issues and current research. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

Epstein, S., & Feist, G. J. (1988). Relation between self- and other-ac- ceptance and its moderation by identification. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 309-315.

Feeney, J. A., & Noller, P. (1990). Attachment style as a predictor of adult romantic relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psy- chology, 58, 281-291.

Fey, W E (1955). Acceptance by others and its relation to acceptance of self and others: A revaluation. Journal of Abnormal Social Psychol- ogy, 50, 274-276.

George, C, Kaplan, N., & Main, M. (1987). The adult attachment inter- view. Unpublished manuscript. University of California, Berkeley.

Greenwald, A. G. (1982). Self and memory. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), Psy- chology oflearning and motivation (Vol.15, pp. 535-553). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

Griffin, D., & Bartholomew, K. (1991). A latent variable analysis of adult attachment styles. Unpublished manuscript.

Grossmann, K., Fremmer-Bombik, E., Rudolf, J., & Grossmann, K. E. (1988). Maternal attachment representations as related to patterns of infant-mother attachment and maternal care during the first year. In R. A. Hinde & J. Stevenson-Hinde (Eds.), Relationships within families (pp. 241-260). Oxford, England: Clarendon Press.

Hazan, C, & Shaver, P. (1987). Conceptualizing romantic love as an attachment process. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 511-524.

Horowitz, L. M, Rosenberg, S. E., Baer, B. A., Ureno, G., & Villasenor, Y S. (1988). Inventory of Interpersonal Problems: Psychometric properties and clinical applications. Journal of Consulting and Clini- cal Psychology, 56, 885-892.

Horowitz, L. M., Rosenberg, S. E., Ureno, G., Kalehzan, B. M., &

ATTACHMENT STYLES 243

O’Halloran, P. (1989). Psychodynamic formulation, consensual re- sponse method, and interpersonal problems. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 57, 599-606.

Horowitz, L. M., Wright, J. C, Lowenstein, E., & Parad, H. W (1981). The prototype as a construct in abnormal psychology: 1. A method for deriving prototypes. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 90, 568- 574.

Kaplan, N., & Main, M. (1985, April). Internal representations of at- tachment at six years as indicated by family drawings and verbal responses to imagined separations. In M. Main (Chair), Attachment: A move to the level of representation. Symposium conducted at the meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Toronto, Ontario, Canada.

Kiesler, D. J. (1983). The 1982 interpersonal circle: A taxonomy for complementarity in human transactions. Psychological Review, 90, 185-214.

Kobak, R. R., & Sceery, A. (1988). Attachment in late adolescence: Working models, affect regulation and representations of self and others. Child Development, 59,135-146.

Kruskal, J. B., & Wish, M. (1978). Multidimensional scaling(Sage Uni- versity paper series on Quantitative Applications in the Social Sciences, No. 07-011). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.

Kruskal, J. B., Young, F. W, & Seery, J. B. (1973). How to use KYST, a very flexible program to do multidimensional scaling and unfolding. (Available from Bell Laboratories, 600 Mountain Avenue, Murray Hill, NJ 07974).

Lamb, M. E., Thompson, R. A., Gardner, W R, Charnov, E. L., & Estes, D. (1985). Security of infantile attachment as assessed in the “strange situation”: Its study and biological interpretation. In S. Chess & A. Thomas (Eds.), Annual progress in child psychiatry and child develop- ment, 1985 (pp. 53-114). New York: Brunner/Mazel.

Latty-Mann, H., & Davis, K. E. (1988, November). Adult children of alcoholics (ACOAs): Family dynamics and patterns of romantic relat- ing. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the National Council on Family Relations, Philadelphia.

Main, M., & Goldwyn, R. (1988). An adult attachment classification system. Unpublished manuscript, University of California, Depart- ment of Psychology, Berkeley.

Main, M., Kaplan, N.3 & Cassidy, J. (1985). Security in infancy, child- hood, and adulthood: A move to the level of representation. In I. Bretherton & E. Waters (Eds.), Growing points in attachment theory and research. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Devel- opment, 50 (1-2, Serial No. 209), 66-106.

Main, M., & Solomon, J. (1990). Procedures for identifying infants as disorganized/disoriented during the Ainsworth Strange Situation. In M. X Greenberg, D. Cicchetti, & E. M. Cummings (Eds}, Attach- ment in the preschool years: Theory, research, and intervention. Chi- cago: University of Chicago Press.

Paddock, J. R., & Nowicki, S. (1986). An examination of the Leary circumplex through the Interpersonal Check List. Journal of Re- search in Personality, 20,107-144.

Phillips, E. L. (1951). Attitudes toward self and others: A brief ques- tionnaire report. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 15, 79-81.

Quinton, D., Rutter, M., & Liddle, C. (1984). Institutional rearing, par- enting difficulties, and marital support. Psychological Medicine, 14, 107-124.

Ricks, M. (1985). The social transmission of parental behavior: At- tachment across generations. In I. Bretherton & E. Waters (Eds.), Growing points in attachment theory and research, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 50, 211 -230.

Rinn, J. L. (1965). Structure of phenomenal domains. Psychological Review, 72, 445-466.

Rosch, E. (1978). Principles of categorization. In E. Rosch & B. B. Lloyd (Eds.), Cognition and categorization. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self-image. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

Rutter, M. (1988). Functions and consequences of relationships; Some psychopathological considerations. In R. A. Hinde & J. Stevenson- Hinde (Eds.), Relationships within families. Oxford, England: Clar- endon Press.

Schaefer, E. S., & Bayley, N. (1963). Consistency of maternal behavior from infancy to preadolescence. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 61,1-6.

Sroufe, L. A., & Fleeson, J. (1986). Attachment and the construction of relationships. In W Hartup & Z. Rubin (Eds}, Relationships and development (pp. 51-71). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Sroufe, L. A., & Waters, E. (1977). Attachment as an organizational construct. Child Development, 48,1184-1199.

Strack, S. (1987). Development and validation of an adjective check list to assess the Millon personality types in a normal population. Jour- nal of Personality Assessment, 51, 572-587.

Swann, W B. (1983). Self-verification: Bringing social reality into har- mony with the self. In J. Suls and A. G. Greenwald (Eds}, Psychologi- cal perspectives on the self (Vol. 2, pp. 33-66). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Swann, W B. (1987). Identity negotiation: Where two roads meet. Jour- nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53,1038-1051.

Troy, M., & Sroufe, L. A. (1987). Victimization among preschoolers: Role of attachment relationship history. Journal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 26,166-172.

Wiggins, J. S. (1979). A psychological taxonomy of trait-descriptive terms: The interpersonal domain. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 395-412.

Wiggins, J. S., Phillips, N, & Trapnell, P. (1989). Circular reasoning about interpersonal behavior: Evidence concerning some untested assumptions underlying diagnostic classifications. Journal of Person- ality and Social Psychology, 56, 296-305.

Wiggins, J. S., & Pincus, A. L. (1989). Conceptions of personality dis- orders and dimensions of personality. Psychological Assessment: A Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 1, 305-316.

Wiggins, J. S., Steiger, J. H., & Gaelick, L. (1981). Evaluating circum- plexity in personality data. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 16, 263-289.

(Appendixes follow on next page)

244 KIM BARTHOLOMEW AND LEONARD M. HOROWITZ

Appendix A

Study 1 Interview Rating Scales

1. Elaboration (9-point scale, a = .88) refers to how forthcoming subjects were in the interviews, including the degree to which specific memories were recounted to support generalizations, depth versus su- perficiality of self- and other-descriptions, and emotional content.

2. Coherence (9-point scale, a = .68) refers to the degree to which subjects presented a coherent, integrated, and internally consistent portrayal of their experiences and feelings in close relationships.

3. Self-disclosure in friendships (7-point bipolar scale, a = .87). A score of 1 indicates a complete avoidance of self-disclosure, 4 indicates open disclosure with discretion, and 7 indicates indiscriminate and excessive self-disclosure.

4. Intimacy of friendships (7-point scale, « = .79) rates the degree of intimacy evident in platonic friendships.

5. Balance of control in friendships (5-point bipolar scale, a = .55). A 1 indicates that the subjects were much more involved, more compro- mising, and less in control of the course of their relationships than social partners, a 3 indicates ideal mutuality, and a 5 indicates that the subjects were more in control than social partners.

6. Highest level previous involvement in romantic relationships (5- point scale, a = .96). The scale ranged from no previous involvements (I) to some dating (2) to one or more short-term, nonserious relationships (3) to at least one long-term, serious relationship (4) to a serious relationship involving a long-term commitment (5).

7. Balance of control in romantic relationships (5-point bipolar scale, a = .79). See (5) above.

8. Self-confidence (5-point scale, a = .84) rates social self-confidence. 9. Emotional expressiveness (7-point bipolar scale, a =.82). Al indi-

cates an extremely rigid overcontrolled emotional style, a 4 indicates

the capacity for open emotional expression coupled with the ability to control emotions when appropriate, and a 7 indicates an undercon- trolled histrionic style.

10. Crying frequency (5-point scale, a = .93). The scale ranges from almost never, less than once per year (1) through average frequency, sometimes (3) to constant, almost daily (5).

11. Warmth (5-point scale, a = .72). A 1 indicates extreme interper- sonal coldness, a 3 indicates an average level of warmth, and a 5 indi- cates an exceptional degree of warmth.

12. Reliance on others (7-point bipolar scale, a = .81). This scale ranges from compulsive self-reliance (1) to excessive emotional depen- dence on others (7). The midpoint of 4 indicates the ability to rely comfortably on others when appropriate, coupled with the capacity to be self-reliant.

13. Use of others as a secure base when upset (5-point scale, a = .83). Scores range from never goes to others when upset (1) through sometimes goes to others, but also uses other coping mechanisms (3) to always goes to others, principal response to any upset (5).

14. Nonsocial versus social crying (5-point bipolar scale, a = .95). A1 indicates crying only when alone, a 3 indicates an equal likelihood of crying alone or with others, and a 5 indicates crying almost exclusively in the presence of others.

15. Caregiving (7-point bipolar scale, a = .62). This scale assesses the tendency to look after others in personal relationships. A 1 indi- cates extreme antipathy to looking after others or having others rely on one, a 4 indicates comfort with caregiving in appropriate situations without any tendency to seek out others to look after, and a 7 indicates an excessive need to look after others to the point of martyrdom.

Appendix B

Self-Report Attachment Style Prototypes

Secure. It is relatively easy for me to become emotionally close to others. I am comfortable depending on others and having others de- pend on me. I don’t worry about being alone or having others not accept me.

Dismissing. I am comfortable without close emotional relation- ships. It is very important to me to feel independent and self-sufficient, and I prefer not to depend on others or have others depend on me.

Preoccupied. I want to be completely emotionally intimate with others, but I often find that others are reluctant to get as close as I would like. I am uncomfortable being without close relationships, but I some- times worry that others don’t value me as much as I value them.

Fearful. I am somewhat uncomfortable getting close to others. I want emotionally close relationships, but I find it difficult to trust others completely, or to depend on them. I sometimes worry that I will be hurt if I allow myself to become too close to others. Note. In subsequent revisions, the word relatively has been deleted from the secure prototype and the words somewhat and sometimes have been deleted from the fearful prototype.

Received July 24,1990 Revision received March 8,1991

Accepted March 22,1991 •

"Order a similar paper and get 15% discount on your first order with us
Use the following coupon
"FIRST15"

Order Now